Skip to main content

The effect of anxiety and autism symptom severity on restricted and repetitive behaviors over time in children with fragile X syndrome

Abstract

Background

Restricted and repetitive behaviors (RRBs) are highly prevalent and reduce function in individuals with fragile X syndrome (FXS). As transdiagnostic features of intellectual disability, elevated rates of RRBs in FXS could represent various underlying known co-occurring conditions in FXS such as anxiety or autism spectrum disorder (ASD), yet this distinction has not been investigated. Further, delineating whether RRBs are more indicative of anxiety or ASD in FXS may clarify phenotypic profiles within FXS and improve differential assessment.

Methods

We longitudinally examined the potentially independent or multiplicative effect of ASD and anxiety symptom severity on RRBs in 60 children with FXS. Anxiety was measured using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), ASD severity was measured using the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS), and RRBs were measured using the Repetitive Behavior Scale – Revised (RBS-R). We estimated a series of moderated regression models with anxiety and ASD symptoms at the initial assessment (Time 1) as predictors of RRBs at the outcome assessment two years later (Time 2), along with an anxiety-by-ASD interaction term to determine the potential multiplicative effect of these co-occurring conditions on RRBs.

Results

Results identified a significant interaction between ASD and anxiety symptom severity at the initial assessment that predicted elevated sensory-motor RRBs two years later. Increased sensory-motor RRBs were predicted by elevated ASD symptoms only when anxiety symptom severity was low. Likewise, increased sensory-motor RRBs were predicted by elevated anxiety symptoms only when ASD symptom severity was low. Interestingly, this relationship was isolated to Sensory-Motor RRBs, with evidence that it could also apply to total RRBs.

Conclusions

Findings suggest that ASD and anxiety exert independent and differential effects on Sensory-Motor RRBs when at high severity levels and a multiplicative effect when at moderate levels, which has important implications for early and targeted interventions.

Background

Fragile X syndrome (FXS) is the most common inherited cause of intellectual disability (ID) and the leading known single-gene cause of autism, with rates of FXS occurring in approximately 1 in 7,000 males and 1 in 11,000 females [38]. FXS is a single-gene disorder caused by a CGG repeat expansion on the Fragile X Messenger Ribonucleoprotein 1 (FMR1) gene on the X chromosome, resulting in reduced production of Fragile X Messenger Ribonucleoprotein Protein (FMRP), which is a protein that is crucial to brain development [30]. Typically, males with FXS are more severely affected than females due to the protective or compensatory effect of the second unaffected X chromosome in females [31], though they present with a more variable profile than males [16, 18]. FXS is characterized by a complex behavioral phenotype that includes moderate ID (typically ranging from moderate to severe ID in males and no ID to moderate ID in females [16, 18], impairments in language and communication [1], anxiety [15, 23], elevated physiological arousal [37, 44], self-injurious and aggressive behavior [17],and autistic-like symptoms such as social avoidance, poor eye contact/gaze aversion [14, 64, 65] and restricted and repetitive behaviors (RRBs,e.g., hand-flapping, verbal perseveration, [55]. In addition to these discrete phenotypic features, individuals with FXS are at increased risk for specific comorbid disorders, most notably anxiety and autism spectrum disorder (ASD). There is substantial overlap in symptomatology specific to ASD and anxiety, as well as symptomology that could be independent of ASD and anxiety as part of the general FXS phenotype. Thus, the FXS phenotype is highly complex and includes features of multiple co-morbid conditions that emerge and likely interact across development. The complexity of the FXS phenotype poses substantial challenges for accurate and early diagnosis critical to optimizing outcomes in children with FXS. As such, studies focused on disentangling the relationships among multiple features of the FXS phenotype employing a developmental framework are critical. The present study focused on delineating the role of autism and anxiety features on RRBs in FXS over time, which has the potential to inform intervention planning.

Restricted and repetitive behaviors in FXS

Restricted and repetitive behaviors (RRBs) are nearly universal in FXS [55]. RRBs are a broad class of behaviors that includes sensory-motor behaviors (e.g., hand-flapping), self-injurious behavior (SIB) (e.g., biting oneself), restricted interests (e.g., preoccupation with one subject or activity), compulsive behavior (e.g., arranging/ordering, closing open doors), and ritualistic/sameness behavior (“insistence on sameness”,e.g., resists changing activities). RRBs can often be classified into “lower-order” types that involve repetitive motor movements or self-injury, as well as into “higher-order” types that are more complex and cognitively mediated, such as ritualistic/sameness behavior or compulsive behavior or restricted interests [10]. Although this classification can be useful in characterizing the form and topography of RRBs, these two broad categories may potentially obscure more nuanced differences in the underlying function or contributing mechanisms [54, 59].

Although RRBs can be considered a transdiagnostic or common marker for various neurological and neurodevelopmental disorders [41, 50], RRBs are elevated in FXS in comparison to other genetic syndromes including those associated with ID [55]. Up to 98% of males with FXS display repetitive motor movements/stereotypy [36], 79% of males display SIB [36], 73% of males display repetitive questions, repetitive speech, or restricted conversation about the future [82], and 72% of males and 55% of females exhibit compulsive behavior [33]. Given that most males with FXS show a more severe clinical presentation than females, research on RRBs in females with FXS is lacking, with the limited research showing that females with FXS are rated as exhibiting less severe RRBs than males with FXS, with the exception of Compulsive behaviors [54, 61]. Nevertheless, across both males and females with FXS, higher rates of RRBs are associated with poorer outcomes in FXS including greater cognitive impairment, lower adaptive behavior skills, and higher rates of problem behavior [61]. Accordingly, it is essential to identify the key factors contributing to RRBs in FXS to attenuate their potential associated consequences; yet, despite the high prevalence of RRBs in FXS overall, these currently remain poorly understood.

Many core and associated features of the FXS phenotype have been implicated in the presence and severity of RRBs. For instance, nonverbal IQ at Time 1 was shown to predict Restricted Interests in boys with FXS at Time 2 [59], and nonverbal IQ has been found to longitudinally predict compulsive behavior in both males and females with FXS (ages 1–18 years old, [54]). Further, Abbeduto and colleagues [3] found that RRB severity was best predicted by psychiatric symptoms consistent with anxiety, social avoidance, and manic/hyperactive behavior in adolescent and young adult males with FXS. The substantial overlap in symptomatology between anxiety, ASD, and the FXS phenotype introduces complexity in delineating the specific underlying function of RRBs in FXS. The possibility that anxiety may drive autistic features in FXS [51, 78] exacerbates the FXS phenotypic complexity and poses a challenge in disentangling the various factors that may contribute to these clinical phenotypic features. While anxiety has been found to predict some types of RRBs (Restricted Interests, Compulsive, and Ritualistic/Sameness behavior) in males with FXS [59], no studies have examined the longitudinal relationship between anxiety and RRBs in both males and females with FXS while also taking autism symptomatology into account, as the present study does. Considering the high but variable presentation of RRBs in FXS and their association with greater levels of functional impairment [61], distinguishing the specific contributing factors of various RRB subtypes can better inform phenotypic characterization and targeted treatment [59].

Autism in FXS

ASD is highly prevalent in FXS and may differentially or multiplicatively account for the presence or severity of RRBs. Specifically, 54–80% of males and 14–41% of females meet diagnostic criteria for ASD (e.g., [3, 45, 46]). More recently, 60.7% of preschoolers with FXS were found to meet diagnostic criteria for ASD (73% of boys and 28.6% of girls), even when employing expert clinical judgement to distinguish ASD from developmental delay, anxiety, or ADHD [63], reducing the potential for inflated prevalence which can occur when using cutoff scores in isolation. Because RRBs are a core diagnostic domain for ASD [5], the presence of comorbid ASD in FXS may also contribute to elevated RRBs independent of, or in conjunction with, co-occurring anxiety.

Prior evidence has identified behavior-specific associations between RRBs and ASD in FXS. Specifically, some studies show that individuals with FXS and comorbid ASD are no more likely to show certain types of RRBs (e.g., compulsive behavior and self-injury) than those with FXS without ASD [33, 75], suggesting that ASD symptomatology may not be the primary driver of RRBs in FXS [59]. However, other types of RRBs (sensory-motor behaviors, restricted interests, behavioral inflexibility) do appear at higher rates in children with comorbid FXS+ASD compared to children with FXS without ASD [35, 61]. Recent evidence also shows that the severity of RRB subtypes in early development differentiates the presence of co-occurring autism in FXS from FXS-only in middle childhood. Specifically, severity of sensory-motor behavior, restricted interests, self-injury, and compulsive behavior all significantly predicted later ASD diagnostic outcomes in children with FXS [63], suggesting that the underlying existence of ASD as a developmental liability in FXS may contribute to RRBs. Thus, the severity of ASD features in FXS may either contribute to or exacerbate existing RRBs in FXS.

However, it is important to note that, although compulsive behavior, insistence-on-sameness behaviors, and total level of RRBs on the Repetitive Behaviour Questionnaire (RBQ) were found to significantly relate to ASD symptomology on the Autism Screening Questionnaire (ASQ) in males with FXS [55], at the item level, only one item on the RBQ (“Does your child insist on doing things in a certain way or redoing things until they are “just right”?) was significantly correlated with the ASQ. Moss et al., [55] interpreted this finding to suggest that RRBs in FXS “are not entirely related to autism spectrum phenomenology.” Coupled with the transdiagnostic nature of RRBs within ID, this evidence suggests that alternative mechanisms – such as anxiety – may account for RRBs in FXS in addition to (or instead of) autism symptomatology.

Anxiety in FXS

Anxiety is one of the most prominent characteristics of FXS [15] and the most common symptom treated in FXS [6]. Indeed, individuals with FXS are more likely to meet criteria for anxiety disorders than other ID groups or the general population [15]. Although females with FXS have (on average) higher cognitive and verbal abilities abilities than males with FXS, as well as lower rates of ASD symptomatology than males, the spectrum of anxiety-related behaviors is quite similar overall in both sexes [48], with approximately 52–86% of males with FXS and 77% of females with FXS aged 5 to 26 years meeting diagnostic criteria for at least one anxiety disorder [15, 23]. Even 39% of preschoolers with FXS meet criteria for at least one anxiety disorder [63], though rates differ based on age, sex, cognitive ability, and the measure(s) used.

Due to the large incidence of co-occurring anxiety in FXS, it is possible that RRBs may function as a coping strategy for anxiety in FXS – that is, anxiety may cause or at least contribute to RRBs in FXS. Indeed, at least one type of RRB – verbal perseveration, considered a hallmark of FXS – has often been attributed to the excessive anxiety that is a classic feature of the FXS behavioral phenotype [8, 26, 49, 57]. In support of this notion, Crawford and colleagues [20] recently found that stronger anxiety/stress responses were associated with heightened repetitive language in males with FXS. Further, evidence suggests a long-term association between anxiety and RRBs in males with FXS [59]. Specifically, early anxiety predicted Restricted Interests, Compulsive behaviors, and Ritualistic/Sameness RRBs 18 months later in boys with FXS, suggesting that anxiety could be a causal or at least contributing factor for some types of RRBs in youth with FXS [59]. For instance, 76% of boys with FXS were reported by their parents to exhibit repetitive questions following changes to routines or expectations, with parents reporting that most of these children already knew the answers to the questions that were asked [82]. It is thus possible that RRBs such as repetitive questioning and other types of verbal perseveration observed frequently in FXS may increase predictability (given that the child already knows the answers), thereby reducing anxiety. This repetitive questioning is likely reinforcing to the child because it allows them to escape or avoid the aversive state of anxiety due to changes in routine, thus contributing to further repetitive questioning [82]. However, as with co-occurring ASD in FXS, the phenotypic complexity and multitude of co-morbid conditions in FXS pose challenges to identifying underlying contributors of clinical features such as RRBs, which is essential for improving differential assessment and diagnosis.

Overlapping features

Therefore, although anxiety may contribute to some RRBs in FXS, it is also likely that certain RRBs may not function as a manifestation of anxiety, particularly in the context of the phenotypic complexity of FXS and considering the transdiagnostic nature of RRBs in ID [76]. Accordingly, other factors may account for RRBs in FXS, including co-occurring ASD in isolation, or the interaction between anxiety and ASD in FXS [2, 63, 66, 74]. Evidence for this interaction appears in non-syndromic ASD (nsASD), where, despite RRBs being a core diagnostic feature of ASD, they are associated with increased anxiety symptom severity in this population [67, 70]. Specifically, in youth with nsASD, Rodgers et al. [67] found that those with high anxiety displayed more RRBs than those without anxiety. While an association between general anxiety and overall RRBs has been demonstrated in nsASD, there is also evidence for anxiety relating to specific RRB subtypes [70]. For example, anxiety significantly relates to insistence-on-sameness behaviors in children and adolescents with nsASD as well as the severity of overall RRBs [47, 70].

This pattern of shared (or possibly distinct) mechanistic roles of anxiety or ASD symptomatology in the severity of RRBs may also exist in FXS. For instance, certain anxiety disorders, including Specific Phobia and Social Anxiety, appear at higher rates in individuals with both FXS + ASD versus those without ASD [15], suggesting that when anxiety and ASD co-occur together in FXS, they may exacerbate features of the other. Indeed, comorbid anxiety is associated with increased ASD severity in males with FXS [77]. Further, in addition to leading to increased RRBs, anxiety can present as poor eye contact and social avoidance in FXS [45], which can alternatively translate as greater social impairment associated with ASD [64, 65]. However, additional evidence has differentiated ASD severity from anxiety in adolescent males with FXS [23], as well as in both adults and across childhood [16, 18, 63, 64]. These conflicting findings may be due to differences in the sample (sex, age, IQ), whether assessment of anxiety accounted for intellectual and communication impairments, and that many measures lack the sensitivity necessary to distinguish overlapping symptomatology of anxiety and ASD in FXS. Regardless, the substantial overlap in phenotypic features across anxiety, autism, and FXS complicates early and differential diagnosis, yet the nature of ASD or anxiety as functions of RRBs in FXS remains unclear considering contradictory evidence and the lack of a truly developmental approach in prior research.

The present study

The emergence of anxiety and/or ASD in FXS shapes developmental trajectories and outcomes for children with FXS, regardless of how each may relate to RRBs. Yet, because RRB subtypes may present differently in FXS in the context of increased ASD severity [19], or may reflect increased anxiety independent of ASD [3, 8, 82], disentangling these features as potential mechanisms of RRBs is important for accurate clinical assessment and targeted treatment for youth with FXS. The high prevalence and degree of impairment associated with RRBs in FXS warrants an increased understanding of the functional role anxiety and/or ASD plays in these outcomes within a developmental framework. This effort is particularly critical to effectively identify the timing and targets for intervention. Thus, using a prospective longitudinal design, the present study documents the potentially independent or multiplicative effect of ASD symptom severity and anxiety on the severity of RRB outcomes in both males and females with FXS across development. Specifically, we examine the potential interaction between anxiety and ASD symptomatology at an initial assessment in predicting RRB outcomes 2 years later, controlling for both age and level of RRBs at the initial assessment. Further, we examine individual subtypes of RRBs along with overall RRBs.

Method

Participants

Data for this study was drawn from two independent, but related, studies [withheld for review] as part of larger prospective longitudinal studies on the early development of children with FXS. While multiple papers have been published from these studies [63,64,65, 74, 80], this paper represents a unique set of questions that have never been posed. The current study included data from 60 youth diagnosed with FXS (see Table 1 for descriptive statistics) who had complete data for two assessments. The initial assessment was conducted between 1 and 14 years of age with a mean of 4.95 years (SD = 4.07, Range: 1–13.67). The outcome assessment was conducted approximately two years later with a mean age of 7.69 years (SD = 3.84, Range: 3.56–16.62). Diagnosis of FXS was confirmed through genetic report.

Table 1 Participant demographics and descriptives

Measures

Measurement of restrictive and repetitive behaviors

The Repetitive Behavior Scale – Revised (RBS-R; [12]) was utilized to measure the presence and prevalence of RRBs. The RBS-R is 43-item parent-report questionnaire that assesses RRBs across six subscales on a scale from 0 (behavior does not occur) to 3 (behavior occurs and is a severe problem). The RBS-R was normed on individuals with intellectual disability, and multiple other studies have created different factor structures of repetitive behaviors in individuals with ASD [9]. Consistent with prior work in ASD and FXS [54, 59], we employed the five-factor structure from Bishop et al. [9] that includes: Sensory-Motor behaviors, Restricted Interests, Self-Injurious Behavior, Compulsive Behavior, and Ritualistic/Sameness. These five factors are relatively similar to the six RBS-R subscales, with the biggest change being that the original Ritualistic behavior subscale was combined with the original Sameness subscale into a Ritualistic/Sameness subscale/factor. This 5-factor structure makes more sense conceptually than using the 6 subscales, given that the 2 out of 6 items from the original RBS-R Ritualistic Behavior subscale that loaded onto the Ritualistic/Sameness factor (travel transportation [e.g., insists on taking certain routes/paths; must sit in specific location in vehicles] and play/leisure [insists on certain play activities; insists that other person do certain things during play) both use the phrase “insists on,” which taps into the broader Insistence-on-Samness construct and overlaps with most of the 9 out of 11 items from the original RBS-R Sameness subscale that loaded onto the Ritualistic/Sameness factor (e.g., insists on walking in a particular pattern; insists on sitting at the same place; insists on same routine, household, school, or work schedule every day).

Cognitive ability

Cognitive ability was measured via one of three measures depending on the participant’s age at the time of assessment. The Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL; [56]), Differential Abilities Scale – Second Edition (DAS, [22]), or Leiter-R [68] were utilized to assess the participants’ current cognitive ability.

The MSEL [56] is a developmental measure utilized to assess cognitive abilities in children between 0 and 68 months of age. The MSEL assesses cognitive abilities across five domains: Gross Motor, Fine Motor, Visual Reception, Receptive Language, and Expressive Language. The Early Learning Composite, which is a composite of Fine Motor, Visual Reception, Receptive Language, and Expressive Language, was used as a proxy for IQ. This measure was standardized across 1849 children and demonstrated satisfactory test–retest reliability, internal consistency, and concurrent validity.

The DAS [22] is a measure that assesses cognitive abilities in children between 2:6 and 17:11 years of age. The measure was standardized on a large sample of children (N = 3480), demonstrating excellent internal consistency, satisfactory test–retest reliability, and acceptable concurrent validity. The current study utilized the General Conceptual Ability (GCA) score.

The Leiter-R [68] is an assessment tool used to assess nonverbal intelligence in children between 2 and 20 years 11 months of age. This measure was normed on a sample of 1719 children with acceptable internal consistency, test–retest reliability, and concurrent validity. The Brief-IQ score is created using four subtests: Figure Ground, Form Completion, Sequential Order, and Repeated Patterns.

Each measure of cognitive ability (MSEL, DAS, or Leiter) was utilized to characterize the approximate cognitive level of each participant as appropriate based on age. Cognitive scores were used descriptively to characterize the overall sample, as well as male and female subgroups (see Table 1).

Anxiety

Anxiety symptoms were measured using the DSM Anxiety Problems scale of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; [4]). The CBCL is a parent-report measure that assesses a variety of emotional and behavioral problems in children. Parents completed the version appropriate for their child’s age (CBCL 1.5–5 Years, CBCL 6–18 Years). The DSM Anxiety Problems scale was developed to assess a wide range of symptoms of anxiety including worries, fear of going to school, and clinging to adults. To increase the variability of the sample, raw scores were utilized instead of T scores. (Given the different number of items on the two CBCL scales, a mean score was calculated that reflected the total number of items on the respective scale. Specifically, the total raw score was divided by the total number of items on the age-appropriate scale.)

ASD symptom severity

ASD symptom severity was assessed using the Childhood Autism Rating Scale – Second Edition (CARS-2; [72]), a reliable and valid measure that has been commonly used to measure ASD symptom severity in FXS (e.g., [24, 58, 63,64,65]). The CARS is an observational rating scale completed by a consensus of multiple trained research team members that assess specific features of ASD across 15 symptom areas (e.g., relating to people, adaptation to change). Each item is rated on a scale from 1 to 4, with higher scores indicating more severe symptoms of autism (and total scores above 30 indicating mild-to-moderate symptomology, [25]. Previous research demonstrates that the CARS captures a continuum of autistic behaviors in individuals with a variety of developmental disabilities including FXS (e.g., [43]). The Total Raw Score was used as a primary variable in analyses.

Procedure

The institutional review board at both [withheld for review] and [withheld for review] provided ethical approval. Participants were recruited through local and national organizations serving FXS populations. Data collection occurred in either the research laboratory or the family’s home depending on family preference. Families were provided with monetary compensation and a brief developmental report at the completion of each time point.

Data analyses

All variables were examined for errors and non-normality, and no problems were identified. Descriptive statistics were calculated for all primary variables in the study and are presented in Table 1. The primary aim of the study was to determine whether ASD and anxiety symptom severity exert a mutual or distinct influence on RRBs over time. That is, does ASD or anxiety independently account for the presence of RRBs, or do they interact (exacerbate) one another in a manner that influences degree and severity of RRBs? Accordingly, we estimated a series of moderated regression models with anxiety and ASD symptoms at the initial assessment (Time 1) as predictors of RRBs at the outcome assessment (Time 2), along with an anxiety-by-ASD interaction term to determine the potential multiplicative effect of these co-occurring conditions on RRBs. Additionally, chronological age and RRBs at Time 1 were included as covariates in each model. Separate models were estimated for each of the five RRB factors: Sensory-motor, Restricted Interests, Self-Injury, Compulsive Behavior, and Ritualistic Sameness, as well as a sixth model for RBS Total Score.

Results

To consider moderating effects between anxiety and ASD on later repetitive behaviors, each RRB factor was regressed on T1 anxiety and ASD severity, an interaction between these, and relevant covariates: T1 repetitive behavior factor and T1 chronological age. (See Table 2 for model results).

Table 2 Moderated regression models

For the model predicting outcomes in Sensory-Motor behavior, there was a statistically significant interaction between anxiety and ASD severity, indicating that, as one increases, the effect of the other on Sensory-Motor RRBs over time decreases (b = -0.40; p = 0.024)1. There were no significant effects for the interaction between anxiety and ASD in predicting Restricted Interests, Self-injury, Compulsive behavior, or Ritualistic/Sameness outcomes (see Table 2). The final model testing the interaction effect between anxiety and ASD severity on Total RBS outcomes revealed a trending association (b = -1.19, p = 0.081). This effect was consistent in that increased ASD severity over time was associated with a lessened effect of early anxiety on later total RRBs.

Figure 1 illustrates the interaction effects for Sensory-Motor RRBs and total RRBs over time. Interesting patterns emerged from the interactions such that, at low levels of either anxiety or ASD, the inverse (i.e., high ASD or high anxiety) accounted for elevated Sensory-Motor behaviors. That is, in the context of high anxiety, the effect of ASD severity on repetitive Sensory-Motor behaviors was lessened to the extent that high anxiety appeared to account for elevated levels of Sensory-Motor RRBs. Likewise, at high ASD severity levels but in the context of low anxiety, high ASD severity appeared to account for elevated levels of Sensory-Motor RRBs over time. Additionally, when anxiety and ASD were both estimated at moderate levels, Sensory-Motor RRBs also appeared moderate. Interaction effects for Total RRBs were similar.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Interactions between ASD and Anxiety on Sensory Motor RRBs (Left) and Total RRB (Right) Outcomes. *Note: Effects were trending towards significance for the Total RRBs model (p < 0.10)

Discussion

Results of the present study suggest potentially distinct mechanisms of RRBs in youth with FXS. Our findings illustrate that elevated levels of Sensory-Motor RRBs are predicted by either low ASD symptom severity coupled with high anxiety or, conversely, by high ASD symptom severity coupled with low anxiety (see Fig. 1). Thus, we observed a “see-saw” effect, with elevated ASD symptom severity suppressing the effect of anxiety on Sensory-Motor RRBs and vice-versa (i.e., with elevated autism anxiety symptom severity suppressing the effect of ASD on Sensory-Motor RRBs) across children with FXS. Interestingly, this suggests that either anxiety or autism symptomatology can account for increased levels of Sensory-Motor RRBs over time independently of one another, even when controlling for early RRBs.

Our results differ somewhat from the findings of Oakes et al. [59], who found that anxiety at the initial assessment in 6- to 10-year-old boys with FXS predicted Restricted Interests, Compulsive behaviors, and Ritualistic/Sameness RRBs approximately 18 months later. However, it is important to note that this study [59] did not account for RRBs at the initial assessment, while we did so in the current study. Another reason for our differing findings is that some of the effects of anxiety in the present study are evidently solely accounted for by ASD,when levels of each (anxiety and ASD) are moderate, they are not well-differentiated. Therefore, accounting for the contribution of ASD when examining the relationship between anxiety and RRBs provides a more precise picture of how anxiety specifically impacts RRBs in youth with FXS over time. Our sample also included a wider age range (1–18 years) than Oakes et al., [59] as well as both males and females. Given that the phenotypic profile of RRBs for females with FXS is distinct from that for males [54], with males with FXS rated as having more Sensory-Motor behaviors and females with FXS rated as having more Compulsive behaviors [61], the inclusion of females in our study is also an important consideration for cross-study comparisons.

Our findings that, as ASD severity increased, there was less of an effect of early anxiety on later Sensory-Motor behaviors and overall RRBs (with trends towards significance for overall RRBs) – were somewhat unexpected, considering evidence of the strong co-occurrence between anxiety and ASD in FXS. However, experts in the field debate the validity of ASD diagnoses in FXS [2, 63], see also [32], with some positing that features of ASD are manifestations of co-occurring anxiety in FXS rather than ASD (e.g., [2, 8, 49, 60, 78]). Verbal perseveration, for example, is an overlapping feature between anxiety and ASD, and has been hypothesized to account for an ASD diagnosis in FXS [49]. Moreover, this specific RRB (verbal perseveration) may affect social communication and interaction, further inflating social impairments that may lead to an ASD diagnosis [27, 49]. Arguably, RRBs as a feature of autistic symptomatology in FXS could manifest from attempts to reduce anxiety, rather than as actual evidence for co-occurring ASD. In fact, some researchers even hypothesize that improving anxiety will facilitate improvements in autism severity in FXS [60]. However, the results of our study indicate that the underlying source of RRBs (at least Sensory-Motor RRBs) in FXS varies as a function of the severity of anxiety and ASD symptomatology, suggesting that either ASD or anxiety may drive elevated levels of RRBs rather than both. Our findings align with other recent evidence supporting anxiety and ASD are two distinct disorders that may co-occur independently of or in conjunction with one another in FXS [21, 63, 64, 80]. Ultimately, the present findings provide further contribution to the field by recognizing the complex interplay of both anxiety and ASD symptoms in the prediction of RRBs in FXS.

It appears that, based on the findings of the present study, for youth with more severe ASD, anxiety has less of an effect on later Sensory-Motor behaviors, and, to some extent, overall RRBs, and vice-versa (i.e., for youth with more severe anxiety, ASD symptom severity has less of an effect on Sensory-Motor RRBs, and to some extent, overall RRBs). This could be a measurement artifact due to “diagnostic overshadowing,” which refers to the propensity for parents or practitioners to attribute symptoms of anxiety to the autism itself [62]. In this way, symptoms of anxiety in youth with FXS might be overlooked or unnoticed because they are overshadowed by high levels of autism symptomatology. Conversely, it is also possible that high levels of anxiety could overshadow symptoms of ASD in FXS so that, for youth with higher levels of anxiety, autism symptoms are not as noticeable and therefore are perceived to have less of an impact on Sensory-Motor behaviors or overall RRBs.

In considering that anxiety and ASD may independently contribute to specific RRBs, as our findings suggest, this could mean there are two different mechanisms or pathways to Sensory-Motor RRBs in youth with FXS: one through anxiety, and the other through autism. As such, it is important to also consider the underlying function specific RRBs may serve. For instance, children with FXS may engage in motor stereotypies as a means of self-regulation or self-soothing to mitigate anxiety [40, 70], or as an enjoyable activity, as a means of “stimming” or ego syntonic behavior [29]. Using an applied behavior analysis (ABA) framework, RRBs may function to provide automatic negative reinforcement in the context of anxiety (reducing anxiety) or provide automatic positive reinforcement in the context of ASD (increasing pleasurable sensory input). Whereas Compulsive and Ritualistic/Sameness behaviors both likely function to avoid, escape, or reduce anxiety, Sensory-motor RRBs could serve both functions,some children may perform sensory-motor behaviors (e.g., body-rocking) to engage in calming activities when their anxiety or arousal levels become too high in order to bring them down to a manageable level, whereas others may engage in sensory-motor RRBs if their arousal levels are too low in order to bring them up to an optimal level.

Because different RRBs may serve different functions, treatment approaches should also differ accordingly to maximize treatment efficacy. This has significant implications for the development of specific behavioral interventions for children with FXS, which are already somewhat limited in application (but see [34]). Indeed, research has demonstrated that behavioral interventions for challenging behaviors in individuals with developmental disabilities are twice as likely to be effective if they are based on a functional assessment of the factors that evoke and maintain the behavior than if they are not (e.g., [13]). The substantial within-syndrome variability in the form and function of RRBs in FXS [64, 65] necessitates individualized assessment and intervention [52].

Restricted interests, compulsive, self-injurious, and ritualistic-sameness behavior

Although we found that, as ASD severity increases, the effect of early anxiety on later Sensory-Motor behaviors lessens, we did not find a significant main effect or interaction effect for anxiety and ASD predicting Restricted Interests, Compulsive behavior, Self-injury, or Ritualistic-sameness behavior. This is somewhat surprising for Compulsive and Ritualistic-sameness behavior, given previous cross-sectional findings that scores on the Compulsive behavior subscale, Insistence-on-sameness subscale, and total RRB scores were significantly correlated with autism symptomatology for males with FXS [55]. However, there are several possible explanations for the lack of significant effects. First, it is possible that Sensory-Motor behaviors were more likely to show significant effects because they (along with Restricted Interests) are the most problematic types of RRBs in both males and females with FXS [59, 61]. Second, it is possible that we did not find significant effects for these subtypes because the majority of research examining RRBs in FXS only includes males, whereas our sample encompasses the full FXS phenotype including both males and females, each of whom exhibit a different profile and trajectory of RRBs. Third, there may not have been significant findings for Restricted Interests, Compulsive behavior, Self-injury, or Ritualistic-sameness behavior because we examined them at the factor/subtype level rather than at the item level. Previous research suggests that specific items on RRB measures may differentiate individuals with FXS more so than subscales/subtypes [3, 55]. For example, in past research only one individual item from the Insistence-on-sameness subscale of the RBQ – “just right” behavior – was significantly correlated with ASD symptomatology [55], suggesting that many RRBs commonly observed in individuals with FXS may not be associated with other symptoms of ASD [59]. Finally, the lack of significant main effects for Restricted Interests, Compulsive and/or Ritualistic-Sameness behavior may be accounted for by a lack of sensitivity in the RBS-R for detecting verbal perseveration, which tends to be highly elevated in FXS [49]. Increased sensitivity in measures of RRBs in FXS may be necessary to detect a significant association between anxiety and verbal ritualistic behavior.

Limitations and future directions

The present study had several strengths including our relatively large sample of youth with FXS (N = 60, including both males and females), the prospective longitudinal nature of the study, which is scarce in FXS, and the use of a validated factor structure in RRBs. However, this study also has several limitations. First, relying on parent-reported measures alone may possibly limit the scope of anxiety and RRBs detected in youth with low cognitive and/or verbal abilities, particularly in the case of anxiety that presents atypically [23]. That is, cognitive ability and communication skills may be limited in FXS and obscure anxiety or perceived RRBs, or may contribute to increased subtlety in some RRBs as well as manifestations of anxiety; thus, parents may have difficulty discerning whether certain behaviors relate to RRBs or anxiety. This was possibly compounded by using an anxiety measure normed on neurotypical youth (the CBCL). Although the CBCL has been widely used to assess anxiety in individuals with ASD (e.g., [7]) and FXS (e.g., [64, 80]), some of the standard parent-report anxiety questionnaires designed for neurotypical populations (e.g., CBCL) have reduced sensitivity to detect clinical anxiety in ASD, particularly in children with ID [42]. Third, the lack of a comparison group limits our ability to determine the specificity of these findings to FXS. Fourth, our study lacked purely objective measures of RRBs (such as wireless sensors that automatically recognize stereotypical motor movements [28], somewhat limiting our ability to fully differentiate RRBs within our measure of ASD severity. However, it is worth noting that the CARS captures a wide variety of phenotypic features of ASD, only few of which are specific to RRBs, supporting our findings that severity of ASD features contributes to the development of RRBs in some capacity. Further, as previously noted, RRBs are a transdiagnostic symptom in ID [41]. Finally, although a strength of this manuscript is the inclusion of both males and females with FXS, it is important to keep in mind that the “full fragile X phenotype” is different for males and females. That said, we analyzed the data with males only to examine whether the relational patterns seen were driven by sex, and the pattern of findings for males only did not differ meaningfully from the results for the combined group. (The sample size for the females precluded analyzing the data from females separately.)

To address these few limitations, future work should aim to include direct behavioral observations of RRBs and anxiety as part of a multi-informant, multi-method assessment to supplement parent reports and corroborate the findings from this study. This approach could capture the duration and intensity of RRBs in addition to the topography, which may better determine whether the intensity of RRBs, rather than presence/absence, in fact differentiates comorbid autism in individuals with FXS and potentially better differentiates anxiety from ASD in FXS. In addition, future research should employ measures that were developed to detect anxiety in youth with neurodevelopmental disorders, such as the Parent-Rated Anxiety Scale for ASD (PRAS-ASD; [71], which was recently created to reduce parents’ reliance on subjective, language-dependent items (e.g., “worries”) and focus more on observable behavioral manifestations of anxiety (e.g., hyperventilates when worried or afraid). Finally, future research should examine potential bidirectional effects between anxiety and RRBs in youth with FXS. After all, while we examined the contribution of early anxiety to later RRBs, some researchers have found that RRBs preceded the onset of anxiety in ASD and therefore conceptualize RRBs as early manifestations of anxiety [7] or as a “response to negative affect, which subsequently increases one’s risk of anxiety over time” [73]. Similarly, while we examined the contribution of ASD symptoms to later RRBs, it could be that high levels of RRBs may – at least in part – account for many of the ASD-like symptoms that are observed in individuals with FXS [81]. Thus, further longitudinal research is needed to disentangle the complex relationships between anxiety, autism symptoms, and RRBs in individuals with FXS.

Conclusion

In sum, we found that anxiety and autism severity each contributed independently to RRBs at elevated levels of symptomatology. Overall, this study improves our understanding of the early risk factors contributing to later RRBs in youth with FXS. Moreover, our results inform the ongoing debate regarding the independence of anxiety and autism in individuals with FXS (e.g., [63, 80]). Although it is often presumed that increased anxiety causes or contributes to the misdiagnosis of ASD in FXS (i.e., the notion that high rates of anxiety make some children with FXS look like they have autism when they really do not), our findings suggest that anxiety and ASD appear to be distinct comorbidities in FXS that contribute to RRBs independent of one another. Ultimately, our findings contribute to an increased understanding of discrepancies in previous studies that implicated only anxiety or ASD in driving RRBs in FXS, given that we examined both anxiety and ASD symptomatology in FXS over time. Interestingly, we found that some types of RRBs – Sensory-motor behaviors – do not appear to be transdiagnostic across severe anxiety and ASD; rather, they appear to be more distinct to either anxiety or autism in youth with FXS, at least when at elevated levels.

Understanding the factors that predict RRBs in youth with FXS has important implications for the creation of targeted treatments for this population. While there is a dearth of intervention research that addresses RRBs in individuals with ASD [79], there are virtually no behavioral intervention studies that target RRBs other than SIB in youth with FXS [52]. Given our findings indicate that anxiety and autism represent two distinct pathways to certain RRBs in youth with FXS, possibly suggesting that RRBs might function to reduce anxiety/arousal for some youth with FXS (anxiety pathway) and to obtain sensory input or increase arousal for others with FXS (autism pathway), interventions that target RRBs in FXS should target these different mechanisms – that is, they should be function-based in an effort to maximize efficacy [39]. If anxiety represents a pathway to RRBs in youth with FXS, then interventions targeting anxiety should be incorporated into function-based treatments for youth with FXS who display RRBs and anxiety. Although there are only a handful of behavioral intervention studies that treat anxiety in individuals with ASD who have a co-occurring ID ([11, 53], see [69] for a review), there have been no behavioral treatment studies for anxiety in individuals with FXS, which represents a significant gap in the literature and a critical area for future research. Overall, with an increased understanding of the role that anxiety and autism symptomatology play in the development of various types of RRBs in FXS, clinicians may be able to focus their interventions to address the true underlying or root cause of RRBs for a particular child.

Availability of data and materials

Data from the findings of this study are available from the authors upon request.

References

  1. Abbeduto L, Brady N, Kover ST. Language development and fragile X syndrome: profiles, syndrome-specificity, and within-syndrome differences. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2007;13(1):36–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Abbeduto L, McDuffie A, Thurman AJ. The fragile X syndrome–autism comorbidity: what do we really know? Front Genet. 2014;16(5):355.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Abbeduto L, Thurman AJ, McDuffie A, Klusek J, Feigles RT, Ted Brown W, Harvey DJ, Adayev T, LaFauci G, Dobkins C, Roberts JE. ASD comorbidity in fragile X syndrome: symptom profile and predictors of symptom severity in adolescent and young adult males. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;15(49):960–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Achenbach TM, Dumenci L, Rescorla LA. Ratings of relations between DSM-IV diagnostic categories and items of the CBCL/6-18, TRF, and YSR. Burlington: University of Vermont; 2001. p. 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  5. American Psychiatric Association DS, American Psychiatric Association DS. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, DC: American psychiatric association; 2013.

  6. Bailey DB Jr, Raspa M, Bishop E, Olmsted M, Mallya UG, Berry-Kravis E. Medication utilization for targeted symptoms in children and adults with fragile X syndrome: US survey. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2012;33(1):62–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Baribeau DA, Vigod S, Pullenayegum E, Kerns CM, Mirenda P, Smith IM, Vaillancourt T, Volden J, Waddell C, Zwaigenbaum L, Bennett T. Repetitive behavior severity as an early indicator of risk for elevated anxiety symptoms in autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020;59(7):890–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Belser RC, Sudhalter V. Arousal difficulties in males with fragile X syndrome: A preliminary report. Developmental Brain Dysfunction. 1995;8(4-6):270–9. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1996-04102-007.

  9. Bishop SL, Hus V, Duncan A, Huerta M, Gotham K, Pickles A, Kreiger A, Buja A, Lund S, Lord C. Subcategories of restricted and repetitive behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43:1287–97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Bishop SL, Richler J, Lord C. Association between restricted and repetitive behaviors and nonverbal IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychol. 2006;12(4–5):247–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Blakeley-Smith A, Meyer AT, Boles RE, Reaven J. Group cognitive behavioural treatment for anxiety in autistic adolescents with intellectual disability: a pilot and feasibility study. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil. 2021;34(3):777–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Bodfish JW, Symons FJ, Parker DE, Lewis MH. Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism: comparisons to mental retardation. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30:237–43.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Carr EG. Positive behavior support for people with developmental disabilities: a research synthesis. Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Retardation; 1999.

  14. Cohen IL, Vietze PM, Sudhalter V, Jenkins EC, Brown WT. Parent-child dyadic gaze patterns in fragile X males and in non-fragile X males with autistic disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1989;30(6):845–56.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Cordeiro L, Ballinger E, Hagerman R, Hessl D. Clinical assessment of DSM-IV anxiety disorders in fragile X syndrome: prevalence and characterization. J Neurodev Disord. 2011;3(1):57–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Crawford H, Abbeduto L, Hall SS, Hardiman R, Hessl D, Roberts JE, Scerif G, Stanfield AC, Turk J, Oliver C. Fragile X syndrome: an overview of cause, characteristics, assessment and management. Paediatr Child Health. 2020a;30(11):400–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Crawford H, Karakatsani E, Singla G, Oliver C. The persistence of self-injurious and aggressive behavior in males with fragile X syndrome over 8 years: a longitudinal study of prevalence and predictive risk markers. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;15(49):2913–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Crawford H, Moss J, Groves L, Dowlen R, Nelson L, Reid D, Oliver C. A behavioural assessment of social anxiety and social motivation in fragile X, Cornelia de Lange and Rubinstein-Taybi syndromes. J Autism Dev Disord. 2020b;50:127–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Crawford H, Moss J, Stinton C, Singla G, Oliver C. Overactivity, impulsivity and repetitive behaviour in males with fragile X syndrome: contrasting developmental trajectories in those with and without elevated autism symptoms. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2018;62(8):672–83.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Crawford H, Oliver C, Groves L, Bradley L, Smith K, Hogan A, Renshaw D, Waite J, Roberts J. Behavioural and physiological indicators of anxiety reflect shared and distinct profiles across individuals with neurogenetic syndromes. Psychiatry Res. 2023;30:115278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Crawford H, Waite J, Oliver C. Diverse profiles of anxiety related disorders in fragile X, Cornelia de Lange and Rubinstein-Taybi syndromes. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47:3728–40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Elliott CD. Differential Ability Scales—Second Edition (DAS-II). San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Ezell J, Hogan A, Fairchild A, Hills K, Klusek J, Abbeduto L, Roberts J. Prevalence and predictors of anxiety disorders in adolescent and adult males with autism spectrum disorder and fragile X syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;15(49):1131–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Fielding-Gebhardt H, Brady N, Bredin-Oja SL, Warren SF. Transactional relationships between maternal requesting and responsivity, adolescent compliance, and task demands in fragile X syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2023;134:104417.

  25. Fielding-Gebhardt H, Warren SF, Brady NC. Developmental trajectory of communication repair in children with fragile X syndrome. Autism Dev Lang Impair. 2020;5:2396941520909014.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Friedman L, Sterling A, Barton-Hulsey A. Gaze avoidance and perseverative language in fragile X syndrome and autism spectrum disorder: brief report. Dev Neurorehabil. 2018;21(2):137–40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Friedman L, Lorang E, Hilvert E, Sterling A. “Are we done yet?” Question-asking in boys with fragile X syndrome and idiopathic autism spectrum disorder. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2020;63(6):1822-34.

  28. Goodwin MS, Haghighi M, Tang Q, Akcakaya M, Erdogmus D, Intille S. Moving towards a real-time system for automatically recognizing stereotypical motor movements in individuals on the autism spectrum using wireless accelerometry. In: Proceedings of the 2014 ACM international joint conference on pervasive and ubiquitous computing 2014. p. 861–872.

  29. Guertin EL, Vause T, Thomson KM, Frijters JC, Feldman MA. Obsessive–compulsive behaviors in autism spectrum disorder: behavior analytic conceptual frameworks. Behav Anal. 2022;22(1):81.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Hagerman R, Lauterborn J, Au J, Berry-Kravis E. Fragile X syndrome and targeted treatment trials. Modeling fragile X syndrome. Results Probl Cell Differ. 2012;54:297–335.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Hagerman RJ, Hagerman PJ, editors. Fragile X syndrome: Diagnosis, treatment, and research. US: Taylor & Francis; 2002.

  32. Hall SS, Lightbody AA, Hirt M, Rezvani A, Reiss AL. Autism in fragile X syndrome: a category mistake? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(9):921–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Hall SS, Lightbody AA, Reiss AL. Compulsive, self-injurious, and autistic behavior in children and adolescents with fragile X syndrome. Am J Ment Retard. 2008;113(1):44–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Hall SS, Monlux KD, Rodriguez AB, Jo B, Pollard JS. Telehealth-enabled behavioral treatment for problem behaviors in boys with fragile X syndrome: a randomized controlled trial. J Neurodev Disord. 2020;12:1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Harrop C, Dallman AR, Lecavalier L, Bodfish JW, Boyd BA. Behavioral inflexibility across two neurogenetic conditions: down syndrome and fragile x syndrome. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2021;126(5):409–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Hessl D, Tassone F, Cordeiro L, Koldewyn K, McCormick C, Green C, Wegelin J, Yuhas J, Hagerman RJ. Brief report: aggression and stereotypic behavior in males with fragile X syndrome—Moderating secondary genes in a “single gene” disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:184–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Hogan A, Hunt E, Smith K, Black C, Bangert K, Klusek J, Roberts J. Trajectories of heart activity across infancy to early childhood differentially predict autism and anxiety symptoms in fragile X syndrome. Front Psych. 2021;6(12):727559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Hunter J, Rivero-Arias O, Angelov A, Kim E, Fotheringham I, Leal J. Epidemiology of fragile X syndrome: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Med Genet A. 2014;164(7):1648–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Ingram K, Lewis-Palmer T, Sugai G. Function-based intervention planning: comparing the effectiveness of FBA function-based and non—function-based intervention plans. J Posit Behav Interv. 2005;7(4):224–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Joosten AV, Bundy AC, Einfeld SL. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation for stereotypic and repetitive behavior. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39:521–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Kästel IS, Vllasaliu L, Wellnitz S, Cholemkery H, Freitag CM, Bast N. Repetitive behavior in children and adolescents: Psychometric properties of the German version of the repetitive behavior scale-revised. J Autism Dev Disord. 2021;51:1224–37.

  42. Kerns CM, Winder-Patel B, Iosif AM, Nordahl CW, Heath B, Solomon M, Amaral DG. Clinically significant anxiety in children with autism spectrum disorder and varied intellectual functioning. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2021;50(6):780–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Klusek J, Hunt AW, Mirrett PL, Hatton DD, Hooper SR, Roberts JE, Bailey DB. Reading and phonological skills in boys with fragile X syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:1699–711.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Klusek J, Martin GE, Losh M. Physiological arousal in autism and fragile X syndrome: Group comparisons and links with pragmatic language. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2013;118(6):475–95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Klusek J, Martin GE, Losh M. Consistency between research and clinical diagnoses of autism among boys and girls with fragile X syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2014;58(10):940–52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Lee M, Martin GE, Berry-Kravis E, Losh M. A developmental, longitudinal investigation of autism phenotypic profiles in fragile X syndrome. J Neurodev Disord. 2016;8:1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Lidstone J, Uljarević M, Sullivan J, Rodgers J, McConachie H, Freeston M, Le Couteur A, Prior M, Leekam S. Relations among restricted and repetitive behaviors, anxiety and sensory features in children with autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2014;8(2):82–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Lozano R, Thompson T, Dixon-Weber J, Erickson CA, Berry-Kravis E, Williams S, Smith E, Frazier JA, Rosselot H, Farmer C, Hessl D. Observable symptoms of anxiety in individuals with fragile X syndrome: parent and caregiver perspectives. Genes. 2022;13(9):1660.

  49. Martin GE, Roberts JE, Helm-Estabrooks N, Sideris J, Vanderbilt J, Moskowitz L. Perseveration in the connected speech of boys with fragile X syndrome with and without autism spectrum disorder. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2012;117(5):384–99.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Martínez-González AE, Piqueras JA. Validation of the Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised in Spanish-speakers participants with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48:198–208.

  51. McDuffie A, Abbeduto L, Lewis P, Kover S, Kim JS, Weber A, Brown WT. Autism spectrum disorder in children and adolescents with fragile X syndrome: within-syndrome differences and age-related changes. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2010;115(4):307–26.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Moskowitz LJ, Jones EA. Uncovering the evidence for behavioral interventions with individuals with fragile X syndrome: A systematic review. Res Dev Disabil. 2015;38:223–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Moskowitz LJ, Walsh CE, Mulder E, McLaughlin DM, Hajcak G, Carr EG, Zarcone JR. Intervention for anxiety and problem behavior in children with autism spectrum disorder and intellectual disability. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47:3930–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Moskowitz LJ, Will EA, Black CJ, Roberts JE. Restricted and repetitive behaviors in males and females with fragile X syndrome: developmental trajectories in toddlers through young adults. J Autism Dev Disord. 2020;50:3957–66.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Moss J, Oliver C, Arron K, Burbidge C, Berg K. The prevalence and phenomenology of repetitive behavior in genetic syndromes. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39:572–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Mullen EM. Mullen scales of early learning. Circle Pines: AGS; 1995. p. 58–64.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Murphy MM, Abbeduto L. Gender differences in repetitive language in fragile X syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2007;51(5):387–400.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. Neal CN, Brady NC, Fleming KK. Narrative analysis in adolescents with fragile X syndrome. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2022;127(1):11–28.

  59. Oakes A, Thurman AJ, McDuffie A, Bullard LM, Hagerman RJ, Abbeduto L. Characterising repetitive behaviours in young boys with fragile X syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2016;60(1):54–67.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  60. Rajaratnam A, Potter LA, Biag HM, Schneider A, Petrasic IC, Hagerman RJ. Review of autism profiles and response to sertraline in fragile X syndrome-associated autism vs. non-syndromic autism; next steps for targeted treatment. Front Neurol. 2020;11:581429.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Reisinger DL, Shaffer RC, Tartaglia N, Berry-Kravis E, Erickson CA. Delineating repetitive behavior profiles across the lifespan in fragile X syndrome. Brain Sci. 2020;10(4):239.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  62. Reiss S, Levitan GW, Szyszko J. Emotional disturbance and mental retardation: diagnostic overshadowing. Am J Ment Defic. 1982;86(6):567-74.

  63. Roberts JE, Bradshaw J, Will E, Hogan AL, McQuillin S, Hills K. Emergence and rate of autism in fragile X syndrome across the first years of life. Dev Psychopathol. 2020;32(4):1335–52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Roberts JE, Crawford H, Will EA, Hogan AL, McQuillin S, Tonnsen BL, O’Connor S, Roberts DA, Brewe AM. Infant social avoidance predicts autism but not anxiety in fragile X syndrome. Front Psych. 2019a;7(10):199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Roberts J, Crawford H, Hogan AL, Fairchild A, Tonnsen B, Brewe A, O’Connor S, Roberts DA, Abbeduto L. Social avoidance emerges in infancy and persists into adulthood in fragile X syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019b;49:3753–66.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Roberts JE, Ezell JE, Fairchild AJ, Klusek J, Thurman AJ, McDuffie A, Abbeduto L. Biobehavioral composite of social aspects of anxiety in young adults with fragile X syndrome contrasted to autism spectrum disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2018;177(7):665–75.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Rodgers J, Glod M, Connolly B, McConachie H. The relationship between anxiety and repetitive behaviours in autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42:2404–9.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  68. Roid GH, Miller LJ. Leiter international performance scale-revised (Leiter-R). Wood Dale: Stoelting; 1997. p. 10.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Rosen TE, Connell JE, Kerns CM. A review of behavioral interventions for anxiety-related behaviors in lower-functioning individuals with autism. Behav Interv. 2016;31(2):120–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Russell KM, Frost KM, Ingersoll B. The relationship between subtypes of repetitive behaviors and anxiety in children with autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2019;62:48–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Scahill L, Lecavalier L, Schultz RT, Evans AN, Maddox B, Pritchett J, Herrington J, Gillespie S, Miller J, Amoss RT, Aman MG. Development of the parent-rated anxiety scale for youth with autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2019;58(9):887–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Schopler E, Van Bourgondien ME, Wellman GJ, Love SR. Childhood Autism Rating Scale–Second Edition (CARS-2). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2010.

  73. Sellick T, Ure A, Williams K. Repetitive and restricted behaviours and anxiety in autism spectrum disorder: protocol for a systematic review and meta-analysis. Syst Rev. 2021;10:1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Smith K, Hogan AL, Will E, Roberts JE. Attention bias and prodromal anxiety symptoms in toddlers with fragile X syndrome and down syndrome. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2021;126(2):167–81.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Symons FJ, Clark RD, Hatton DD, Skinner M, Bailey DB Jr. Self-injurious behavior in young boys with fragile X syndrome. Am J Med Genet A. 2003;118(2):115–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Symons FJ, Sperry LA, Dropik PL, Bodfish J. The early development of stereotypy and self-injury: a review of research methods. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2005;49(2):144–58.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  77. Talisa VB, Boyle L, Crafa D, Kaufmann WE. Autism and anxiety in males with fragile X syndrome: an exploratory analysis of neurobehavioral profiles from a parent survey. Am J Med Genet A. 2014;164(5):1198–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Thurman AJ, McDuffie A, Hagerman R, Abbeduto L. Psychiatric symptoms in boys with fragile X syndrome: a comparison with nonsyndromic autism spectrum disorder. Res Dev Disabil. 2014;35(5):1072–86.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Uljarević M, Arnott B, Carrington SJ, Meins E, Fernyhough C, McConachie H, Le Couteur A, Leekam SR. Development of restricted and repetitive behaviors from 15 to 77 months: stability of two distinct subtypes? Dev Psychol. 2017;53(10):1859.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Wall CA, Hogan AL, Will EA, McQuillin S, Kelleher BL, Roberts JE. Early negative affect in males and females with fragile X syndrome: implications for anxiety and autism. J Neurodev Disord. 2019;11(1):1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Wolff JJ, Bodfish JW, Hazlett HC, Lightbody AA, Reiss AL, Piven J. Evidence of a distinct behavioral phenotype in young boys with fragile X syndrome and autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51(12):1324–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Woodcock K, Oliver C, Humphreys G. Associations between repetitive questioning, resistance to change, temper outbursts and anxiety in Prader-Willi and Fragile-X syndromes. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2009;53(3):265–78.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge all of the families that have generously given their time to participate in this study.

Funding

This work was supported by the NIMH (PI: Jane Roberts, grant numbers R01MH90194; R01MH090194; 1R01MH107573) and the NICHD (PI: Elizabeth Will, grant number 1K99HD105980-01; PI: Donald Bailey, grant number P30-HD003110-35).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

LJM conceptualized the paper, wrote the initial draft of the paper, interpreted the data, and reviewed and revised the paper. EAW performed the statistical analysis, interpreted the data, wrote the Results section, created the Figures, and revised the paper. CJB participated in conducting the statistical analysis, created the tables, and drafted the Methods section. JER served as the principal investigator of the parent study; she conceived of and designed the larger longitudinal study from which the data were drawn, participated in the design and interpretation of the data for this paper, and revised the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jane E. Roberts.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical approval was obtained from both the University of South Carolina and the University of North Carolina Institutional Review Board. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. Written consent was obtained from participants’ parents or guardians before data were collected.

Consent for publication

All participants consented to have their anonymized data published.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing or conflicts of interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Moskowitz, L.J., Will, E.A., Black, C.J. et al. The effect of anxiety and autism symptom severity on restricted and repetitive behaviors over time in children with fragile X syndrome. J Neurodevelop Disord 16, 61 (2024). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s11689-024-09569-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s11689-024-09569-2

Keywords