Skip to main content

Differences in sleep EEG coherence and spindle metrics in toddlers with and without receptive/expressive language delay: a prospective observational study

Abstract

Background

Changes in brain connectivity during development are thought to reflect organizational and maturational processes that correspond to skill acquisition in domains like motor, language, and cognition. This theory is supported by findings in typically developing children as well as observations of abnormal connectivity among children with neurodevelopmental differences. However, few coherence studies have capitalized on the potential of sleep electroencephalogram (EEG) to examine the developing brain, especially among very young children for whom formal neurodevelopmental diagnosis is not yet possible. Sleep microarchitecture in young children may offer key insights into neurophysiological abnormalities associated with neurodevelopmental trajectories and potentially aid in early detection and intervention. In this study, we explored sleep EEG coherence and sleep spindles in typically developing toddlers and toddlers at increased risk of later neurodevelopmental diagnoses.

Methods

We investigated EEG coherence and sleep spindles in 16 toddlers with receptive and expressive language delay (LangD) and 39 typically developing (TD) toddlers. Participants were aged 12–22 months at baseline, and 34 (LangD, n=11; TD, n=23) participants were evaluated again at 36 months of age.

Results

Average EEG coherence was stronger in the LangD group than the TD group, with differences most prominent during slow-wave sleep. Some age-related increases in coherence were observed, but these did not differ between groups. Sleep spindle density, duration, and frequency changed between baseline and follow-up for both groups, with the LangD group demonstrating a smaller magnitude of change than the TD group. The direction of change was frequency band-dependent for both groups.

Conclusions

These findings indicate that atypical sleep EEG connectivity and sleep spindle development can be detected in toddlers at risk of later neurodevelopmental diagnoses.

Trial registration

https://clinicaltrials.gov/study/NCT01339767; Registration date: 4/20/2011.

Background

Electroencephalogram (EEG) is an important tool for studying the developing brain, offering temporal resolution and insight into functional relationships that other methods like magnetic resonance imaging cannot provide. Functional brain connectivity, or the degree of temporal correlation between two spatially distant neuronal populations, informs our understanding of interactions between regions or networks [1]. EEG coherence is a metric of such connectivity and is expressed as the temporal relationship between the phase shift and amplitude ratio of two electrodes [2].

EEG research has demonstrated a relationship between network topology and cortical maturation in newborns [3] and that resting state functional connectivity increases during infancy [4, 5] and early childhood [6]. These changes are thought to reflect organizational and maturational processes that correspond to skill acquisition in developmental domains like motor [7], cognition [8, 9], and language [10]. By the same token, abnormal coherence has been observed among children with a range of neurodevelopmental differences such as learning disorders [11], speech disorders [12], attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [13], and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) [14, 15]. However, the nature of those EEG coherence abnormalities is varied. Perhaps depending on EEG methodology (e.g., calculation of coherence, brain region, frequency band used) or composition of the sample (e.g., age, comorbidities), children with neurodevelopmental differences have been documented to have both more and less coherence than their typically developing counterparts [13, 14].

Importantly, however, most research on the relationship of EEG coherence to neurodevelopmental differences has been conducted within a (waking) resting state paradigm and in older children/adolescents. This paucity of sleep EEG coherence data in early childhood holds both scientific and logistical promise. Sleep is crucial for the development of physiological and cognitive processes in early childhood. Early on, it plays a key role in building synapses and refining circuits central to motor, language, and social development, and later it becomes essential to learning and memory [16]. In contrast with resting state or task-based paradigms, sleep EEG affords the opportunity to capture neural activity in a way that is unaffected by signal noise from external waking stimuli. This allows for a more sensitive and robust way to image early circuit development, particularly in very young children for whom it may otherwise be difficult. Nevertheless, very few of the many neuroimaging studies investigating the functional and structural underpinnings of neurodevelopmental disorders have capitalized on the potential of sleep EEG to examine the developing brain [15].

Other aspects of sleep EEG may also be related to neurodevelopmental differences. Sleep spindles are a hallmark of NREM sleep presenting as brief bursts of waxing-and-waning oscillations between 10 to 16 Hz [17]. Sleep spindles are believed to have functional roles in information processing, memory consolidation, and neuroplasticity during development [18,19,20]. Because spindle properties are known to change throughout the lifespan, they can provide insight into the neuro-maturational trajectories of individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders [21]. There is preliminary evidence that sleep spindles are related to developmental impairments in older children and adolescents, such as intellectual disability [22] and ASD [23,24,25]. However, due to the paucity of longitudinal data in early childhood, differences in developmental trajectories of sleep spindles in children with and without neurodevelopmental disorders are currently unknown. Furthermore, the relationship between other spindle features such as frequency and chirp (intra-spindle frequency change) to developmental trajectory is largely unexplored. These oscillations reflect thalamo-cortical activity, and their internal frequency variation may hold relevant information for understanding neurophysiology and exploring brain pathology in neurodevelopmental disorders [26]. One recent cross-sectional investigation documented more negative chirp in typically developing children as compared to children with ASD (ages 1 – 8 years) [27], suggesting that this may be a fruitful area of investigation. By examining spindle density, frequency, and chirp in combination, this study aims to provide a more comprehensive assessment of the sleep spindle features in early childhood, facilitating the identification of potential markers for language and cognitive impairments, and contributing to our understanding of the neural substrates underlying these conditions.

In summary, the examination of sleep micro-architecture during the early childhood period could offer key insights into neurophysiological abnormalities that are associated with atypical neurodevelopmental trajectories and could aid in early detection and intervention. However, the existing literature is limited – few EEG studies of coherence and other features during sleep are available for the early childhood period, with or without neurodevelopmental differences. With this study, we aimed to address this paucity of data using a longitudinal natural history study of toddlers with and without receptive and expressive language delay. Language delays are one of the earliest observable manifestations of atypical development in children for whom young age precludes neurodevelopmental diagnoses. In the absence of genetic, congenital, or perinatal cause for concern, language delays are one of the earliest behavioral signs of neurodevelopmental disorder brought to the attention of providers [28]. While language delays in most children resolve without intervention, children with delays in both receptive and expressive language at age 2 years are likelier to experience poorer outcomes [29]. Thus, this subpopulation of toddlers with both receptive and expressive language delay is enriched for later neurodevelopmental disability and therefore presents an opportunity to explore correlates of neurodevelopmental disability before such diagnoses are assignable.

In the current study, we conducted an exploratory analysis of sleep EEG coherence and spindle characteristics among young children with and without receptive and expressive language delay. Given the extant literature, we hypothesized that toddlers with receptive/expressive language delay would exhibit stronger EEG coherence compared to the typically developing toddlers, and that these differences would be most pronounced during slow wave sleep, in comparison to rapid eye movement sleep stage and waking (resting) EEG. Additionally, we predicted that toddlers with language delay would demonstrate lower levels and more stability in sleep spindle density, frequency, and chirp compared to typically developing toddlers.

Methods

Participants

Participants were drawn from a natural history protocol conducted at the NIH Clinical Center in Bethesda, Maryland, USA (protocol number 11-M-0144; NCT01339767). The study was approved by the NIH Institutional Review Board. Consent from the parent or guardian of each child was obtained prior to participation. Inclusion criteria were being between the age of 9 of 21 months, born full-term, no significant medical or motor impairment deemed responsible for the delays (or any known genetic disorder for the typically developing group), and English reported as the primary language spoken in the home. Additionally, at the initial evaluation, eligibility for the language delay (LangD) group included receptive and expressive language T-scores ≥2 standard deviations below average on the Mullen Scales of Early Learning [30] (MSEL), and eligibility for the typically developing (TD) group included T-scores within 1.5 standard deviations of average on all domains of the MSEL.

A total of 62 (LangD, n = 19; TD, n = 43) toddlers enrolled in the natural history study. Participants were included in the current analysis if they had at least one analyzable sleep study, resulting in n=55 toddlers (LangD, n = 16; TD, n = 39) aged 12 to 22 months at their first visit (mean age 17.4±2.8 months) (Table 1). The sample was majority male (n = 34, 62%) and white (n = 36, 65%). Seven (44%) of the LangD participants received a research ASD diagnosis (based on clinical judgment of DSM-5 criteria informed by research-reliable administrations of the Autism Diagnostic Interview and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule) during their study participation, but no other formal diagnoses were systematically evaluated. This small sample size precluded investigation of relationships to eventual ASD diagnosis.

Table 1. Sample characteristics

Procedures

The study design was longitudinal, comprising study visits at 12 and/or 18 months (depending on age of study entry), 24 months, and 36 months. At each study visit, participants received a neurodevelopmental assessment supervised by doctoral-level clinicians, including the MSEL and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, 2nd Edition [31]. Two scores were computed from the MSEL and used in the current analysis: the Verbal Developmental Quotient (VDQ; using the Receptive and Expressive subscales) and the Nonverbal Developmental Quotient (NVDQ; using the Fine Motor and Visual Reception subscales). DQs are analogs of IQs and are calculated by averaging the age equivalents from each subscale, dividing by the chronological age, and multiplying the result by 100. From the Vineland-II, the Socialization standard score was used to reflect a continuous metric of social skills relative to normative expectations. The Socialization score is norm-referenced with a population mean of 100 and SD of 15.

An optional polysomnogram was performed at the participants’ first visit (baseline; BL) and at the 36-month visit (follow-up; FU); those with valid sleep EEG data at either visit were included in the current analysis. A total of 34 (LangD, n=11; TD, n=23) participants had both BL and FU sleep studies, while 21 (LangD, n=5; TD, n=16) had only BL. Digital EEGs were recorded during awake, drowsy, and sleep states using the 10-20 system of electrode placement (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Electrode placement. Red lines indicate electrode pairs pulled for coherence analysis

Analysis

EEG Coherence

Clean 10-minute segments of awake, slow-wave sleep (SWS), and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep were selected for coherence analysis, which was performed masked to participant group using Neuroguide software (Applied Neuroscience Inc., St. Petersburg, FL) with a linked-ear montage.

Electrode pairs (F3-P3, F4-P4, F4-Pz, P3-Fz, P4-Fz, P3-F7, C3-C4, Cz-Pz, F3-F4, Fz-Cz, and P3-P4; Fig. 1) were selected for coherence analysis based on findings from Buckley et al. [32] which found increased coherence in children with ASD compared to typically developing controls, concentrated in frontal-parietal pairs. Frequency bands selected for coherence analysis are Delta (1-4 Hz), Theta (4-8 Hz), Alpha 1 (8-10 Hz), Alpha 2 (10-12 Hz), Beta 1 (12-15 Hz), Beta 2 (15-18 Hz), and Beta 3 (18-25 Hz). Analysis was performed across three sleep stages (awake, REM, SWS), eleven electrode pairs, and seven frequency bands, yielding 231 models.

Coherence values were subjected to Fisher transformation and fixed effects of group (LangD versus TD) and age in months (centered at 18 months) were evaluated with the lme4 [26] package for R version 4.2.1 [33] using a linear mixed model with a random subject-level intercept. These models addressed the questions of whether coherence depended on group, age, or both. False detection rate (FDR) p-value correction was applied to these effects (693 tests in total: three effects within each of 231 models). In separate exploratory analyses, we evaluated the relationship between coherence and behavioral features (MSEL VDQ and NVDQ, and Vineland-II Socialization standard score) by adding the mean-centered behavioral score as a fixed effect. Because these variables are aliased with group membership, we included an interaction between group and behavioral variable, and evaluated the slope of the behavioral variable on coherence within each group to avoid confounding. The parameters of interest in these models were the simple slopes of the behavioral feature within each group. FDR p-value correction was applied to the list of these slopes (462 tests).

Spindle Properties

Both slow spindles (9 Hz and 11 Hz) and fast spindles (13 Hz and 15 Hz) were evaluated separately for density (number of spindles per minute), amplitude, duration, frequency (Hz), and chirp (intra-spindle frequency change). Spindle data were analyzed from all 16 channels using Luna (http://zzz.bwh.harvard.edu/luna/), a software package developed at Harvard Medical School designed to analyze polysomnogram recordings and automate spindle analysis. The Luna software utilized a Morlet wavelet transformation method of spindle detection. Spindle frequency, amplitude, duration, and epoch counts were recorded for each spindle and plotted on a graph for visual analysis and verification.

The effect of age on spindle features was evaluated using linear mixed effects models with fixed effects for study visit (baseline, BL vs. follow-up, FU), group, and sex, and a random subject-level intercept. Differences between LangD and TD group were tested using logistic regression where diagnostic status was a dependent variable and spindle characteristics at each channel as an independent variable with age and sex added as covariates. FDR p-value correction was applied separately for two sets of the analyses: BSL vs FU with mixed effect regression (320 tests in total – 16 channels x 5 spindle metrics x 4 spindle frequencies); LangD vs TD with logistic regression (528 tests in total – 16 channels x 11 spindle metrics at spindle frequencies were significant effect between BSL and FU was observed x 3 conditions BSL, FU, FU-BSL). The relationship of spindle density to behavioral scores were evaluated as described above for coherence.

For all analyses, the p-values were corrected using the FDR method as described above. To make the large number of statistical tests tractable for presentation, we use a graphical approach. The complete set of uncorrected and corrected p-values and test statistics is available as a supplement.

Results

Coherence

Group differences in coherence during awake, REM, and SWS stages are shown in Fig. 2. Increased coherence was observed for LangD relative to TD, with differences being most prominent during SWS. Few differences in coherence between LangD and TD were observed during the awake state. The group-by-age interactions indicated that the effects of group on coherence did not depend on age.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Model-estimated mean coherence at 18 months in the typical group (top row), language delay group (middle row), and the difference between these estimates (bottom row)

The effect of age on coherence is illustrated in Fig. 3. Increased coherence with age was found for four electrode pairs (C3-C4, F4-Pz, Fz-Cz, and P3-Fz), all within the beta 1 frequency (12-15 Hz). Decreased coherence with age was only found for one frequency/pair combination, F3-F4 in the delta frequency.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Model-estimated slope of age on coherence in the full sample (top row) and FDR-corrected p-values for those slopes

In an exploratory analysis, within-group relationships between coherence and behavioral scores (NVDQ, VDQ, and Socialization) were evaluated in separate models. Very few significant relationships between scores and coherence were found for VDQ, NVDQ, or socialization (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Simple slopes of VDQ (Panel A), NVDQ (Panel B), and Socialization (Panel C) on coherence. FDR-corrected p-values < .05 are marked with a black circle

Spindle Properties

Density

In the combined sample, density of slow spindles at 9 Hz decreased from BL to FU in central and temporal channels. Density of slow spindles at 11 Hz increased from BL to FU in frontal channels (Fig. 5). This increase was greater for TD compared to LangD. Furthermore, density of fast spindles at 15 Hz decreased from BL to FU across multiple channels (Fig. 5). At 15 Hz, spindle density was lower for LangD than TD in frontal channels at BL; this difference was smaller in magnitude at FU (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Density of spindles between baseline and follow-up visits. Panel A shows density of spindles between baseline and follow-up for the entire sample. Panel B shows differences in changes in density from baseline to follow-up between LangD and TD for 11 Hz spindles. Panel C shows differences in changes in density from baseline to follow-up between LangD and TD for 15 Hz spindles

As an exploratory analysis, we evaluated the relationship of NVDQ, VDQ, and Socialization to density within each study group. The parameter estimates tended to be negative, associating higher density with lower behavioral scores, but the confidence intervals were wide and contained zero. The single exception was for the relationship of Socialization in the LangD group to spindle density in 15 Hz at the T6 electrode.

Duration

In the combined sample, duration of slow spindles at 11 Hz increased from BL to FU across multiple channels, whereas duration of fast spindles at 15 Hz decreased from BL to FU across multiple channels (Fig. 6). This decrease was lower in magnitude for LangD compared to TD.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Differences in duration of spindles between baseline and follow-up. Panel A shows differences in duration of spindles between baseline and follow-up for the entire sample. Panel B shows differences in changes in duration from baseline to follow-up between LangD and TD for 15 Hz spindles

Frequency

In the combined sample, frequency of slow spindles at 11 Hz increased from BL to FU in posterior channels. Frequency of slow spindles at 9 Hz increased from BL to FU in frontal channels, and this increase was lower in magnitude for LangD compared to TD (Fig. 7) Frequency of fast spindles at 13 Hz decreased from BL to FU across all channels. This decrease was lower in magnitude for LangD compared to TD.

Fig. 7
figure 7

Differences in frequency of spindles between baseline and follow-up. Panel A shows differences in frequency of spindles between baseline and follow-up for the entire sample. Panel B shows differences in frequency from baseline to follow-up between LangD and TD for 9 Hz spindles. Panel C shows differences in frequency from baseline to follow-up between LangD and TD for 13 Hz spindles

Chirp

Chirp of slow spindles at 11 Hz and fast spindles at 13 and 15 Hz decreased from BL to FU, with no difference between TD and LangD (Fig. 8).

Fig. 8
figure 8

Differences in chirp (intra-spindle frequency change) from baseline to follow-up

Discussion

We investigated EEG coherence and sleep spindle properties at two time points in toddlers with and without language delay. Increased coherence found for toddlers with receptive/expressive language delay was state- and stage-dependent, with differences most prominent during SWS in central electrode pairs across frequency bands. The group differences did not depend on age, indicating that any differences between LangD and TD may have already been present prior to toddler age. In exploratory analyses, few associations between continuous behavioral variables and coherence were observed within-group. This suggests that coherence differences are not dimensionally related to severity of neurodevelopmental impairments. An effect of age was observed on spindle properties for both groups; however, the LangD group demonstrated a smaller magnitude of change in spindle density, duration, and frequency in comparison to the TD group.

Interestingly, most of the differences in coherence between LangD and TD were detected in the alpha frequency bands. The alpha band has been implicated in research on neurodevelopmental differences. A previous study by Orekhova et al. [34] found alpha range hyper-connectivity in 14-month-old high-risk infants who were later diagnosed with ASD at 3 years old, suggesting that alpha hyper-connectivity may be an important attribute of ASD neurophysiology. Previous research has indicated that alpha oscillations detected by scalp EEG may contain signals from thalamocortical interactions [35], which has been speculated by Linke et al. [36] to play a crucial role in sleep disruptions in ASD. Alpha oscillations also appear to play an important role in language development and have been found to be correlated with typically developing children’s oral language abilities [35]. In this sample, differences in alpha-frequency coherence between LangD and TD may indicate abnormal thalamocortical functioning in LangD children with increased likelihood for neurodevelopmental disability and may also play a role in differences in language skill and trajectory of attainment.

This was an exploratory analysis intended to generate hypotheses for future confirmatory study, especially in larger samples that will allow for correlation of early sleep EEG characteristics with later diagnostic outcomes. Given that differences in EEG coherence were most prominent during SWS in our sample, future research may be more efficient in focusing primarily on SWS. Furthermore, given our findings regarding differences in sleep spindle properties, future research could investigate whether specific alterations in spindle density, duration, and frequency may predict later neurodevelopmental outcomes, including ASD diagnosis. Although very early ascertainment for later neurodevelopmental differences would be a challenge and require a larger sample size than in the current study, longitudinal analyses encompassing both infancy and childhood would allow for a more developmentally informed understanding of trajectories in coherence and spindle differences. Investigating whether these differences remain stable from early childhood to adulthood would also be illuminating and add to the literature finding cross-sectional differences in older children and adolescents with and without neurodevelopmental diagnoses like ASD.

The current results must be interpreted with respect to several limitations. This was a small study, meaning the power to detect small differences was low, especially given the multiplicity of tests. As such, prospective evaluation of these exploratory results is necessary. The LangD group was ascertained based on both receptive and expressive language delay, which placed them at increased risk of poorer outcomes [29]. Indeed, seven (44%) did go on to receive an ASD diagnosis before the end of study participation. We could not locate epidemiological data with which to compare this rate of ASD diagnosis among children with receptive and expressive language delay in toddlerhood, but it is consistent with increased rates of poorer educational and employment outcomes in later childhood and early adulthood [37]. Regardless, this subset of children who went on to receive an ASD diagnosis was too small for group-level comparison to the remainder of the LangD sample and the TD group, indicating the need for a larger initial sample size.

We note that the inclusion criteria dictated that the LangD sample would have uniformly lower scores on developmental testing than the TD group. However, the TD group in this sample had above-average developmental scores. This may be a function of recruitment practices (e.g., geographical location), but it may also be true that parents with mild concerns would be unlikely to respond to recruitment materials, shifting the average developmental scores in those who did respond. Further, the TD group was on average 2.5 months younger than LangD group at baseline, which could represent a meaningful difference in such young children. These age differences were addressed statistically, but future research with closer age matching may yeild more valid conclusions.

Although the longitudinal nature of this study is a significant strength, there were only two time points and some attrition. We assumed that this missingness was noninformative (i.e., not related to the concepts under study) and that it did not bias our results, but future research with more timepoints and complete data will improve the validity of interpretation. Finally, we note that the study design required that our exploration of dimensional correlates of the EEG parameters be performed within each study group, to guard against errors of inference. However, this reduced the functional sample size and therefore the power of these analyses to detect effects. Future analyses in larger samples that were not ascertained based on the developmental correlates will provide a better test of these questions.

Conclusions

In conclusion, our study of toddlers at risk of later neurodevelopmental diagnoses due to their receptive/expressive language delay reveals early differences in sleep unique electrophysiologic signatures, specifically spindle properties and sleep EEG coherences, which may potentially serve as biomarkers for neurodevelopmental risk. These findings underscore the significance of investigating sleep-related brain activity to understand language development and neurodevelopmental disorders. Sleep EEG presents a promising avenue to elucidate neural connectivity patterns during critical developmental stages, offering unique insights into the functional maturation of brain circuits that influence language processing and cognitive development.

By identifying potential markers for neurodevelopmental differences, our study also highlights the importance of early detection and monitoring. The state- and stage-dependent increases in EEG coherence, particularly during slow-wave sleep, emphasize the relevance of sleep-related brain activity as a window into neural connectivity underlying language delay. Longitudinal assessments indicate that certain neurophysiological differences may be present as early as 18 months, supporting the value of early intervention for children at risk for neurodevelopmental disorders. Nevertheless, given the exploratory nature of this study and small sample size, further research with larger cohorts is necessary to validate and expand on these results. By unraveling the complexities of sleep-related brain activity, we gain insights into the neural substrates shaping language development and neurodevelopmental trajectories, facilitating targeted interventions and support strategies for optimal developmental outcomes.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Abbreviations

EEG:

electroencephalogram

TD:

typically developing

LangD:

language delay

REM:

rapid-eye movement

ASD:

autism spectrum disorder

fMRI:

functional magnetic resonance imaging

NREM:

non-rapid-eye movement

SWS:

slow-wave sleep

NIH:

National Institutes of Health

MSEL:

Mullen Scales of Early Learning

DSM-5:

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition

VDQ:

Verbal Developmental Quotient

NVDQ:

Non-Verbal Developmental Quotient

DQ:

Developmental Quotient

IQ:

Intelligence Quotient

BL:

baseline

References

  1. Friston KJ. Functional and effective connectivity: a review. Brain Connect. 2011;1(1):13–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Srinivasan R, Winter WR, Ding J, Nunez PL. EEG and MEG coherence: measures of functional connectivity at distinct spatial scales of neocortical dynamics. J Neurosci Methods. 2007;166(1):41–52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Tóth B, Urbán G, Háden GP, Márk M, Török M, Stam CJ, et al. Large-scale network organization of EEG functional connectivity in newborn infants. Hum Brain Mapp. 2017;38(8):4019–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Falivene A, Cantiani C, Dondena C, Riboldi EM, Riva V, Piazza C. EEG Functional Connectivity Analysis for the Study of the Brain Maturation in the First Year of Life. Sensors (Basel). 2024;24(15):4979.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Bosch-Bayard J, Biscay RJ, Fernandez T, Otero GA, Ricardo-Garcell J, Aubert-Vazquez E, et al. EEG effective connectivity during the first year of life mirrors brain synaptogenesis, myelination, and early right hemisphere predominance. Neuroimage. 2022;252:119035.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Bathelt J, O’Reilly H, Clayden JD, Cross JH, de Haan M. Functional brain network organisation of children between 2 and 5 years derived from reconstructed activity of cortical sources of high-density EEG recordings. Neuroimage. 2013;82:595–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Bell MA, Fox NA. Crawling experience is related to changes in cortical organization during infancy: evidence from EEG coherence. Dev Psychobiol. 1996;29(7):551–61.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Whedon M, Perry NB, Calkins SD, Bell MA. Changes in frontal EEG coherence across infancy predict cognitive abilities at age 3: The mediating role of attentional control. Dev Psychol. 2016;52(9):1341–52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Bell MA. A psychobiological perspective on working memory performance at 8 months of age. Child Dev. 2012;83(1):251–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Gaudet I, Hüsser A, Vannasing P, Gallagher A. Functional Brain Connectivity of Language Functions in Children Revealed by EEG and MEG: A Systematic Review. Front Hum Neurosci. 2020;14:62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Lizarazu M, Lallier M, Molinaro N, Bourguignon M, Paz-Alonso PM, Lerma-Usabiaga G, et al. Developmental evaluation of atypical auditory sampling in dyslexia: Functional and structural evidence. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36(12):4986–5002.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Vasil’yeva NO, Shmalei SV. Coherent Relations in Ongoing Encephalograms of Preschool Boys with Neurosis-Like Stammering. Neurophysiology. 2013;45(5):468–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Furlong S, Cohen JR, Hopfinger J, Snyder J, Robertson MM, Sheridan MA. Resting-state EEG Connectivity in Young Children with ADHD. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2021;50(6):746–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. O’Reilly C, Lewis JD, Elsabbagh M. Is functional brain connectivity atypical in autism? A systematic review of EEG and MEG studies. PLoS One. 2017;12(5):e0175870.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Schwartz S, Kessler R, Gaughan T, Buckley AW. Electroencephalogram Coherence Patterns in Autism: An Updated Review. Pediatr Neurol. 2017;67:7–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Ahmad SF, Buckley AW, Glaze DG. Neurology of Sleep. Neurol Clin. 2021;39(3):867–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. De Gennaro L, Ferrara M. Sleep spindles: an overview. Sleep Med Rev. 2003;7(5):423–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Cote KA, Epps TM, Campbell KB. The role of the spindle in human information processing of high-intensity stimuli during sleep. J Sleep Res. 2000;9(1):19–26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Fogel SM, Smith CT. The function of the sleep spindle: a physiological index of intelligence and a mechanism for sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(5):1154–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Lindemann C, Ahlbeck J, Bitzenhofer SH, Hanganu-Opatz IL. Spindle Activity Orchestrates Plasticity during Development and Sleep. Neural Plast. 2016;2016:5787423.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Crowley K, Trinder J, Kim Y, Carrington M, Colrain IM. The effects of normal aging on sleep spindle and K-complex production. Clin Neurophysiol. 2002;113(10):1615–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Shibagaki M, Kiyono S, Watanabe K, Hakamada S. Concurrent occurrence of rapid eye movement with spindle burst during nocturnal sleep in mentally retarded children. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1982;53(1):27–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Tessier S, Lambert A, Chicoine M, Scherzer P, Soulières I, Godbout R. Intelligence measures and stage 2 sleep in typically-developing and autistic children. Int J Psychophysiol. 2015;97(1):58–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Farmer CA, Chilakamarri P, Thurm AE, Swedo SE, Holmes GL, Buckley AW. Spindle activity in young children with autism, developmental delay, or typical development. Neurology. 2018;91(2):e112–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Mylonas D, Machado S, Larson O, Patel R, Cox R, Vangel M, et al. Dyscoordination of non-rapid eye movement sleep oscillations in autism spectrum disorder. Sleep. 2022;45(3):zsac010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Schönwald SV, Carvalho DZ, Dellagustin G, de Santa-Helena EL, Gerhardt GJ. Quantifying chirp in sleep spindles. J Neurosci Methods. 2011;197(1):158–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Cumming D, Kozhemiako N, Thurm AE, Farmer CA, Purcell S, Buckley AW. Spindle chirp and other sleep oscillatory features in young children with autism. Sleep Medicine. 2024;119:320–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Hess CR, Landa RJ. Predictive and concurrent validity of parent concern about young children at risk for autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(4):575–84.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Law J, Boyle J, Harris F, Harkness A, Nye C. Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language delay: findings from a systematic review of the literature. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2000;35(2):165–88.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Mullen EM. Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service Inc.; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Sparrow SS, Cicchetti DV. The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Major psychological assessment instruments, Vol 2. Needham Heights, MA, US: Allyn & Bacon; 1989. p. 199–231.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Buckley AW, Scott R, Tyler A, Mahoney JM, Thurm A, Farmer C, et al. State-Dependent Differences in Functional Connectivity in Young Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder. EBioMedicine. 2015;2(12):1905–15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2023.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Orekhova EV, Elsabbagh M, Jones EJ, Dawson G, Charman T, Johnson MH, et al. EEG hyper-connectivity in high-risk infants is associated with later autism. J Neurodev Disord. 2014;6(1):40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Kwok EYL, Cardy JO, Allman BL, Allen P, Herrmann B. Dynamics of spontaneous alpha activity correlate with language ability in young children. Behav Brain Res. 2019;359:56–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Linke AC, Chen B, Olson L, Ibarra C, Fong C, Reynolds S, et al. Sleep Problems in Preschoolers With Autism Spectrum Disorder Are Associated With Sensory Sensitivities and Thalamocortical Overconnectivity. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2023;8(1):21–31.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Nelson HD, Nygren P, Walker M, Panoscha R. Screening for Speech and Language Delay in Preschool Children: Systematic Evidence Review for the US Preventive Services Task Force. Pediatrics. 2006;117(2):e298–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the families for their efforts, and the research team, including Precilla D’Souza and the sleep EEG technicians, for making this effort possible. We thank Judy Mahmalji, MA for assistance in preparing the manuscript.

Funding

Open access funding provided by the National Institutes of Health This research was supported by the intramural research program of the National Institute of Mental Health 1ZICMH002961 and 1ZICMH002962-03 and R03MH108915.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

X.H. contributed to manuscript writing and to data analysis. C.F. and N.K. contributed to manuscript writing, data analysis, and data visualization. L.T. and S.M. assisted in conceptualization and data collection. A.T., G.H., and A.B. contributed to conceptualization of the study and to funding acquisition. A.B. also contributed to manuscript writing, supervised the project, and assisted with overall direction and planning. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ashura Buckley.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by the NIH Institutional Review Board (protocol 11-M-0144; NCT01339767). Consent was obtained from parents or guardians of participants prior to their participation.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ashura Buckley,­ National Institute of Mental Health, 10 Center Drive, Suite 1C250, Bethesda, MD 20892, MD. Email: ashura.buckley@nih.gov.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hong, X., Farmer, C., Kozhemiako, N. et al. Differences in sleep EEG coherence and spindle metrics in toddlers with and without receptive/expressive language delay: a prospective observational study. J Neurodevelop Disord 17, 11 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s11689-024-09586-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s11689-024-09586-1

Keywords