Skip to main content

Brain structure in triple X syndrome: regional gray matter volume and cortical thickness in adult women with 47,XXX karyotype

Abstract

Background

Changes in the brain structure of women with Triple X syndrome (karyotype 47,XXX) have been described in a few studies to date, including reduced total brain volume and regional reductions in gray substance in cortical and subcortical areas. However, the empirical evidence from adults is very limited and group comparison on a voxel-wise basis for gray matter volume and cortical thickness is still missing.

Methods

Using voxel-based morphometry (VBM) and surface-based morphometry (SBM), we investigated regional gray matter changes in a sample of n = 20 adult women (aged 18–49 years) with 47,XXX karyotype using T1-weighted 3T MRI scans.

Results

Compared to an age- and education-matched control group (and controlled for differences in total intracranial volume), the VBM revealed decreased regional gray matter volumes in the hippocampus, amygdala, parts of the basal ganglia, insula, prefrontal areas and cerebellum. To a lesser extent, we also noted specific reductions in cortical thickness in a smaller part of those regions.

Conclusion

The observed network is significantly involved in the processing of cognitive, affective, and social stimuli and might be a potential neuronal correlate of the autism-like social-cognitive problems described in 47,XXX in the literature.

Background

Triple X syndrome (karyotype 47,XXX), also known as trisomy X, is < a sex chromosomal aneuploidy characterized by the presence of an additional X chromosome in females [1]. It has an estimated prevalence of about 0.1% in female newborns [2]. However, only few cases are clinically diagnosed, possibly due to the inconspicuous phenotype and lack of clinical symptoms [3, 4].

Research has identified impairment of different domains comprising motor development, speech and language, educational attainment, socio-economic status and interpersonal relationships in children and adolescents with 47,XXX [5,6,7,8]. Notably, elevated rates of neurodevelopmental conditions like autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), as well as associated socio-cognitive and behavioral problems have been reported [8,9,10,11,12,13]. Individuals with 47,XXX also exhibit heightened susceptibility to (social) anxiety, depression, and psychotic disorders compared to the general population [13,14,15].

In the cognitive domain, mild learning difficulties and executive function deficits are prevalent among those with 47,XXX during childhood and adolescence [16, 17]. Notably, their mean full-scale IQ (FSIQ) tends to be lower, with a leftward shift in the IQ normal curve compared to 46,XX controls, with verbal IQ (VIQ) often more affected [7]. Furthermore, impairments in everyday social functioning, social cognition, and behavior have been observed in both pediatric and adult samples with 47,XXX [18,19,20,21].

A few neuroimaging studies have demonstrated the presence of structural brain abnormalities in individuals with 47,XXX, including alterations in total brain and subcortical volumes, cortical thickness and surface area, and cortical folding in children and adolescents [22,23,24,25,26]. Note that these studies (except for Lenroot et al., 2014) relied on a largely identical group of youth with a mean age of 12 years. In sum, those analyses indicate reduced total brain volume (TBV), and reductions in amygdala and hippocampus gray matter, which for the amygdala was still significant after controlling for reduced TBV [24]. Consistently, in adults, an early study with 10 women revealed initial evidence for smaller amygdala volumes in 47,XXX, although this effect was not significant due to limited statistical power [27]. Beyond these preliminary findings, there is only one study focusing on alterations in cortical, subcortical, and lateral ventricle volumes in an adult sample (Serrarens et al., 2022). Using T1 images acquired with an ultra-high field 7 T scanner, Serrarens and colleagues reported reduced gray matter volumes of the basal ganglia (caudate nucleus, putamen, pallidum, nucleus accumbens), thalamus, and hippocampus, presumably associated with enlarged lateral ventricles. In addition, reduced gray matter was detected in the superior frontal gyrus and superior temporal gyrus in 47,XXX participants compared to controls. Although cortical thickness was not specifically altered in 47,XXX, there was a negative correlation between emotion recognition performance and cortical thickness in the left caudal anterior cingulate cortex and positive with cortical thickness in left lateral occipital, left pericalcarine, and right superior parietal cortex in 47,XXX [28]. In sum, the empirical evidence is currently very limited and heterogeneous (see supplementary Table S1), Hence it is hard to draw a consistent picture based on these very few studies. To put it cautiously, the most consistent finding seems to be reduced total brain volume and reduced gray matter and white matter in subcortical (including amygdala, hippocampus and basal ganglia) and some cortical areas. Initial results also suggest that within group variance in subcortical and cortical structures is related to social functioning.

With the present study, we aimed at replicating reported results in an independent adult sample of women with 47,XXX compared to an age- and education-matched control group. In extending the earlier studies, we took a whole brain voxel-based morphometry (VBM) approach to more precisely localize gray volumetric alterations in 47,XXX and used surface-based morphometry (SBM) to discern potential local alterations in cortical thickness. Finally, we explored potential associations with self-reported traits related to social cognition and psychopathology. Based on the evidence presented above both related to the previous structural imaging studies and phenomenological similarities with ASD, we expected to observe GM volume reductions and reduced cortisol thickness in amygdala, hippocampus, putamen, pallidum, caudate, and thalamus, as well as decreased total brain volume and enlarged ventricles. We exploratively tested associations between regional brain volumes and different self-report measures in the social-cognitive domain such as empathy, autistic traits, as well as symptoms of depression and anxiety.

Methods

Participants

Women with a 47,XXX karyotype were recruited via a German Triplo-X support group, specialists in human genetics, and social media. They were included on the basis of a confirmed 47,XXX karyotype. Controls matched for sex, age and education (highest school-leaving qualification, as detailed in Table S2 provided in the supplementary materials) were recruited via flyers, digital newsletters and local advertising. Participants in both groups were only included if they were 18 years of age or older and spoke the German language at native speaker level. Participants with a history of traumatic brain injury, neurological damage, current intake of psychoactive drugs, magnetic implants in their body, claustrophobia, or pregnancy were excluded from the study. Initially, 54 women diagnosed with 47,XXX karyotype and 64 controls expressed interest in participating. After applying inclusion and exclusion criteria, our final analysis included data from 20 women with 47,XXX karyotype (aged 18–49 years) and 23 controls (aged 18–52 years). The recruitment took place between 2021 and 2023.

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the state medical association Rhineland-Palatinate (#2022–16572). Participants gave written-informed consent before participation. All participants were compensated for their participation and any travel expenses were reimbursed.

Psychological assessment

General intellectual abilities (IQ) were measured with the Culture Fair Test 20 (CFT-20) revised version. The CFT 20-R measures the general intellectual level (basic intelligence) in terms of Cattell’s “General Fluid Ability”. We used part 1 of the German CFT-20-R [29] containing 56 items in four subtests (series continuation, classifications, matrices and topological reasoning). Test-retest reliability for the subtest is rtt = 0.80 and the internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) is α = 0.94. Raw scores were transformed based on the IQ norms provided in the 2nd edition.

Verbal intelligence was estimated via a vocabulary test (WST) [30] designed to measure verbal abilities. The WST consists of 40-word recognition items, each containing one target word and five distractors. The split-half reliability (Spearman-Brown) is r =.95, the internal consistency is α = 0.94. Raw scores were transformed based on the IQ norms provided.

A short version of the Autism Quotient, the AQ-k [31] was used to assess traits related to the autism spectrum. The German short version of the original AQ [32] comprises 33 items rated on 4-point Likert scales and analyzed in dichotomized form, resulting in total scores between 0 and 33 points. The AQ-k has acceptable reliability (internal consistency for the subscales: α = 0.65 − 0.87 and for the total score α = 0.79). In addition, the AQ-k has been shown to distinguish between individuals on the autism spectrum and typically developing individuals with a high sensitivity and specificity at a cutoff score of 17 points [31].

The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) [33] contains 28 items and measures four traits related to empathy: perspective taking (the tendency to adopt others’ viewpoints), fantasy (the tendency to identify with fictional characters in books, movies, and plays), empathic concern (feelings of compassion and concern for others), and personal distress (feelings of discomfort and anxiety in response to others’ distress). For the present analysis, we calculated total scores summing up the scales perspective taking, fantasy, and empathic concern [33]. The internal consistency for the subscales is acceptable and varies between α = 0.68 − 0.79.

Levels of trait anxiety were assessed with the trait scale of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-T) [34]. The trait scale of the STAI assesses individual differences in the tendency to experience and show fear reactions based on 20 items (4-point Likert-scale). The internal consistency is α = 0.90 and the test-retest reliability has been estimated at rtt = 0.67 − 0.90.

The Beck-Depression Inventory (BDI) was used to assess depressive symptoms [35]. Containing 21 items, the BDI assesses the severity of a depression based on the clinical symptoms according to DSM and ICD. The internal consistency has been estimated at approx. α = 0.90 in different samples and the test-retest reliability rtt = 0.78 in a community sample.

Magnetic resonance imaging

T1-weighted images were acquired on a 3T Philips Ingenia Elition X equipped with a 15-channel head coil. Sagittal images covering the whole head were acquired with an 3D MPRAGE sequence with the following parameters: 208 sagittal slices (256 × 256 in-plane resolution), 1 mm isotropic voxels, flip angle = 8°, TR = 5.22 ms, TE = 2.36 ms. Total scan duration was approx. 6:30 min.

Morphometric analysis

We applied four different analyses pipelines to the structural images: (a) Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis focusing on voxel-wise gray matter volume differences, (b) a respective region-based analysis focusing on regional gray matter volume (i.e., a voxel-based ROI analysis), (c) a surface-based morphometry (SBM) analysis focusing on point-wise cortical thickness, and (d) a corresponding region-based analysis focusing on regional cortical thickness (i.e., a surface-based ROI analysis).

Processing of images

For further analyses, T1 images were spatially preprocessed and segmented with the Computational Anatomic Toolbox (CAT12.9, 2550;, 36) implemented in Statistic Parametric Mapping (SPM12, 7771; https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/) running on Matlab (R2018a). In a first step, T1 images were normalized and segmented into gray matter, white matter, and CSF by using the probability maps as priors provided within CAT12. As a result of segmentation, bias, noise and global intensity corrected, modulated and normalized gray and white matter images were saved for regional volumetric analysis. For quality control, we calculated deviations in individual gray matter image intensities from the global mean. Those images deviating from the mean with > 1.5 IQR were visually inspected for artefacts (n = 3). None of the scans had to be excluded, as all scans were of good data quality. Images were then smoothed with a 3D Gaussian kernel of 6 mm FWHM for VBM, and with a kernel of 20 mm FWHM for SBM. We also calculated individual total intracranial volumes (TIV) for group comparison and to be included as a covariate in the general linear model for group comparison of regional gray matter volumes, in both VBM and ROI-based analyses.

Voxel-based morphometry

For group comparison of VBM regional gray matter volume, we set up a GLM with one factor (group) and age and TIV as covariates. After estimation, two contrasts were defined to test for regional gray matter volume decrease vs. increase in the 47,XXX group. The resulting t-maps were written with at a peak-voxel threshold of p < .05 with a family-wise error (FWE) correction based on random field theory [37]. A cluster extent threshold of k > 100 was also applied. Automatic anatomical labelling (AAL3v1) was used to label peak voxels in cortical and subcortical regions [38]. The VBM outcomes were overlaid onto coronal sections of MNI152 average template using MRIcroGL (v1.2; 39).

Voxel-based ROI-analysis

The voxel-based ROI analysis relied on Neuromorphometrics atlas (http://neuromorphometrics.com/) to calculate regional gray matter, white matter and ventricular volumes. The labeling based on the Neuromorphometric atlas implemented in CAT12 differentiates and labels 136 regions including ventricles, vessels, and total volumes of the CSF, cerebrum, and cerebellum. Results on gray matter volumes were tested for significance with an FDR-corrected threshold of p < .05 and projected on the Freesurfer average template (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) within CAT12.

Surface-based morphometry

Surface data to estimate regional cortical thickness were written during segmentation within CAT12, see above. Similar to the VBM analysis, we set up a GLM with group as the only factor, but without controlling for TIV. After estimation, again two contrasts were defined to test for group differences in regional cortical thickness. The resulting t-maps were thresholded at p < .05 (FWE-corrected) and at an exploratory threshold of p < .001 (uncorrected).

Surface-based ROI analysis

The surface-based ROI analysis employed the DK40 atlas [40] to calculate regional cortical thickness for predefined ROIs. Those results were projected on the Freesurfer average template (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) with a Holm-Bonferroni corrected threshold of p < .05.

Correlation analysis

To explore potential associations between regional GM volume and autistic and empathic traits on the one hand and anxiety/depressive symptoms on the other hand, we calculated Pearson’s correlation coefficients between AQ, IRI, BDI and STAI scores with regional GM volumes for bilateral amygdala, hippocampus, putamen, and cerebellum based in the voxel-based ROI analysis described above. Correlation coefficients were calculated for the two study groups separately.

Results

Study group characteristics

Study groups did not differ significantly in age, education, general and verbal IQ, empathy, trait anxiety or depression (all p > .10) but 47,XXX exhibited more pronounced autistic traits (p < .05; Cohen’s d = 0.82) - see Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic and psychometric characteristics

Voxel-based morphometry

Total intracranial volume and ventricles

TIV was significantly lower in 47,XXX women (m = 1317, SD = 85 cm3) than in controls (m = 1403, SD = 75 cm3), t(41)= 3.505, p < .001. In addition, the group difference in total ventricle volume approached significance after controlling for TIV (F[1,40] = 3.95, p =.054), with 47,XXX women showing slightly enlarged ventricle volumes (m = 15.3, SD = 6.5 cm3) than controls (m = 13.4, SD = 4.4 cm3).

Gray matter volume

For the contrast (controls > 47,XXX) representing decreased GM volume in women with 47,XXX, we found significant GM volume reductions in four extended clusters comprising [1] left hippocampus, amygdala, and insula (k = 3851; x, y,z=-29, -6, -20; T = 11.29; p < .000001) [2], the bilateral cerebellum (k = 4991; x, y,z = 14, -72, -41; T = 10.43 p < .000001) [3], right amygdala (k = 892; x, y,z = 27, -6, -15;T = 9.48; p < .00001) [4], right putamen and insula (k = 1680; x, y,z,=35, 0, -3; T = 8.73; p < .0001) - see the first four clusters in Table 2; Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Results of voxel-based morphometry (VBM). Statistical parametric maps (SPM) are projected on coronal slices of the MNI152 template illustrating regions with reduced GM volume in women with a 47,XXX karyotype. (a) Extended clusters showing gray matter volume reduction comprising hippocampus, amygdala, putamen, insula. (b) Extended clusters in the cerebellum. Results were FWE-corrected with a voxel threshold of T > 6.14; p < .05

In addition, we identified smaller, scattered clusters in the cerebellum, inferior frontal cortex, mainly in the orbitofrontal cortex, and the fusiform gyrus (see Table 2 lower part). Notably, in the reverse contrast testing for increased GM volume in the 47,XXX group compared to controls, none of the voxels were significant.

Table 2 VBM GLM results for the contrast controls > 47,xxx. Significant clusters with decreased GM volume in 47,XXX at a voxel threshold of p < .05 (FWE-corr.) and a cluster size threshold of k > 100. A complete list of significant clusters can be found in supplementary table S3

In addition to the voxel-wise analysis, region-of-interest (ROI)-based analysis revealed corresponding results: We identified significant ROIs with reduced gray matter volume in (all cluster p < .05 Holm-Bonferroni-correction): Amygdala, Hippocampus, Insula, Putamen, Thalamus, orbitofrontal cortex, fusiform gyrus, parts of the temporal lobe, frontal cortex – see Fig. 2; Table 3. Robust effects were observed in limbic areas (Cohen’s d > 1.35), especially in the bilateral amygdala (Cohen’s d = 2.38 and d = 3.51, respectively). Again, reverse comparison testing for increased regional gray matter volume in the 47,XXX group revealed no significant ROI. None of the regional gray matter volumes in amygdala, hippocampus and putamen were associated with lateral ventricle volume (all correlations with p > .05).

Fig. 2
figure 2

ROI-based analysis showing significant ROI of decreased gray matter volume in women with 47,XXX compared to controls. ROIs were defined using the Neuromorphometrics atlas. Results were FDR-corrected at p < .05

Table 3 Results of ROI-analysis for the contrast controls > 47,xxx. Significant rois with D.creased GM volume in 47,XXX (all p < .05; Holm-Bonferroni-corr.). Effect sizes are reported as Cohen’s D

Surface-based morphometry

Cortical thickness

The contrast controls > 47.XXX for point estimate of cortical thickness revealed no significant voxels at p < .05 (FWE-corr.). At a more liberal, explorative level of p < .001 (uncorr.), we detected three clusters of reduced cortical thickness in 47,XXX: left visual cortex (BA 17/18, k = 96; x, y,z=-16,-83,-14; T = 4.19; Z = 3.79; p =.000074), entorhinal cortex (BA 28; k = 46; x, y,z=-27,-4,-27; T = 4.01; Z = 3.65; p =.00014), right inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44/45; k = 49; x, y,z = 49,-11,-16; T = 3.59; Z = 3.32; p =.00045) – red clusters in Fig. 3. The reverse contrast of increased cortical thickness in 47,XXX again revealed no significant cluster at p < .05 (FWE-corr.), but three clusters at p < .001 (uncorrected): supplementary motor area (BA 6; k = 49; x, y,z = 26,-21,69; T = 4.13; Z = 3.75; p =.00009), right inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44/45; k = 45; x, y,z = 39,1,4; T = 3.75; Z = 3.45; p =.00028) and right mid temporal gyrus (BA 21; k = 15; x, y,z = 54,-30,-20; T = 3.74; Z = 3.44; p =.00029)– blue clusters in Fig. 3.

The ROI-based analysis revealed two significant ROIs (Holm-Bonferroni corrected) of decreased cortical thickness in 47,XXX women: the left fusiform gyrus (T = 2.79; Z = 2.65; p =.0358; Cohen’s d = 0.85) and the right rostral anterior cingulate cortex (T = 1.72; Z = 1.76; p =.0470; Cohen’s d = 0.53) – see Fig. 4.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Point-wise cortical thickness analysis showing significant clusters of altered cortical thickness in women with 47,XXX compared to controls. Blue clusters indicate increased vs. red areas indicate reduced cortical thickness in 47,XXX. Results are presented uncorrected at p < .05 for illustrative purpose

Fig. 4
figure 4

ROI-based thickness analysis showing significant ROI of decreased cortical thickness in women with 47,XXX compared to controls. ROIs were defined based on the DK40 atlas. The results were Holm-Bonferroni corrected and thresholded at p < .05

Associations between brain morphology and psychometric characteristics

Explorative correlation analyses revealed significant correlations between bilateral amygdala/hippocampus GM volumes and self-reported empathy (IRI), anxiety (STAI-T) and depression (BDI) in women with 47,XXX (see Table 4). Higher regional GM volumes in amygdala and hippocampus was associated with higher levels of self-reported empathy, and lower levels of anxiety and depression. In controls, none of these correlations were significant (all r <.40; all p > .05). After controlling for age and TIV, only the correlations between the left amygdala and STAI, right amygdala and STAI and BDI, and right hippocampus and STAI remained significant. A complete partial correlation matrix (controlled for age and TIV) for both groups is included in the supplementary materials, see Table S4.

Table 4 Correlations between autistic traits (AQ), empathy (IRI), anxiety (STAI-T), and depression (BDI) and regional GM volume in selected rois in women with 47,XXX; asterisks indicate significant Pearson’s correlations (uncorrected)

Discussion

Controlled studies investigating brain structure in girls with a 47,XXX karyotype are scarce, and even fewer in adults. With the present study, we compared a group of 20 adult women with 47,XXX with an age- and education-matched control group regarding regional gray matter volume and regional cortical thickness. VBM analysis revealed marked reductions of gray matter volume in the inferior and medial temporal lobe including the hippocampus, amygdala, and fusiform regions, medial frontal and orbital regions, bilateral insula, basal ganglia including putamen and caudate, and several clusters in the cerebellum. SBM analysis revealed cortical thickness reduction in some of these regions, however these clusters were less extended, and the effects were considerably smaller in magnitude compared to the VBM results. Overall, these regional effects were robust after controlling for TIV, which was reduced in the 47,XXX group. As expected, the 47,XXX group exhibited lower TIV, slightly enlarged ventricles, higher levels of self-reported autistic traits, but comparable intellectual abilities and normal levels of self-reported anxiety and depression. Exploratively, we observed that amygdala/hippocampus GM volume was a positive predictor for self-reported empathy and a negative predictor for anxiety and depression in women with 47,XXX. Although significant, albeit moderate, we observed correlations specifically in the 47,XXX group which should be interpreted very cautiously as structural neural alterations could predispose to social-cognitive problems. Further conclusions would require large-scale correlative studies, ideally with a longitudinal design.

Reduced gray matter volume in the basal ganglia has been reported in girls with 47,XXX [26] and in adult women [28]. While these studies reported volume reductions in different parts of the basal ganglia (predominantly in pallidum and caudate), we observed decreased regional gray matter volume in the right putamen and caudate. Given that these regions are involved in a wide range of cognitive, affective and motor processes, the potential functional impact of the structural alterations we observed remains speculative. However, given the reported impairments in cognition, affect, and motor skills in 47,XXX (e.g. 3, 7, 9, 14, 18) and the fact that structural anomalies have been reported in the basal ganglia in ASD [41, 42], it seems plausible that these structural changes could also play a role in the development of the social cognitive impairments described for 47,XXX.

The first remarkable finding of the present study is the strong effect detected in the bilateral medial temporal lobe, including volume reduction in hippocampus, amygdala, and parahippocampal gyrus. Reduced amygdala volume in 47,XXX has been reported in girls [24] and, as a trend, also in adult women [27]. However, a recent study failed to confirm those results in adults [28]. Interpreting these contradictory findings is complicated by the fact that very different methodologies were applied to obtain the volumetric data in small subject samples. Patwardhan and colleagues manually segmented the amygdala and hippocampus, while Nadig et al. and Serrasens et al. used different automated procedures to determine ROIs. Although manual segmentation by trained raters is in principle a precise and reliable method, the statistical power (n = 10) in the study by Patwardhan et al. (2002) [27] was limited. In addition, since 1.5 Tesla T1-weighted scans were used, the contrast may have been limited when segmenting the amygdala and hippocampus. In the study by Serrarens et al. (2022) [28], segmentation was performed with Freesurfer and volumes were estimated for predefined ROIs. While in this study sample size was larger and the T1 scans used were presumably of higher quality (scanned at 7 Tesla), the ROI-only approach may be less sensitive at detecting effects in the amygdala. In our study, taking a voxel-based approach and applying advanced segmentation algorithms in CAT12, we were able to detect decreased regional volumes in the hippocampus and amygdala, both within an extensive cluster with local peaks in the hippocampus and amygdala. It seems possible that the diverse gray matter segmentation and estimation algorithms applied in different studies may explain subtle differences in the amygdala. However, the effects may simply have been larger in our study sample.

As part of a larger network (the so-called “social brain”), the amygdala plays an essential role in the representation of emotional processes and is of central importance for social-cognitive functions [43,44,45]. We chose to focus on autism in this discussion due to the previously reported autism-like features in 47,XXX [9, 10, 13], and the fact that in our study women with 47,XXX reported higher levels of autistic traits. Accordingly, recent reviews have come to the conclusion that structural alterations and functional impairments in the amygdala and adjected areas could be the basis of socio-cognitive deficits within the autism spectrum (e.g. 46). Indeed, recent meta-analytic evidence supports the hypothesis that gray matter volume reductions in the amygdala are associated with autism [47]. In this respect, the regionally reduced gray-matter volume in these core regions within the social brain network detected in our sample could also be a pathophysiological factor related to the reported social-cognitive deficits and social difficulties [19, 28, 48] and autism-like features [9, 10] in adult women with 47,XXX. It should be noted that amygdala volume reductions have also been reported in association with other psychiatric conditions, including emotional instability [49], schizophrenia [50], and depression [51], and might therefore be a relatively unspecific phenomenon. However, changes in amygdala structure are also associated with socio-affective problems in these psychiatric disorders, although the exact relationship is not yet well understood.

The amygdala and the hippocampus are parts of the neural circuits underlying cognitive and affective aspects of empathy [52, 53] particularly in processing emotional stimuli, attributing mental states to others, and emotional memory [54]. Both cognitive and affective facets of empathy are affected in autism spectrum disorder [55, 56]. Reduced amygdala volume may contribute to difficulties in recognizing and responding to others’ emotions [54, 57], while grey matter volume reductions the hippocampus might affect the contextualization of social information [58]. In our sample, the positive correlation between amygdala/hippocampus GM volume and self-reported empathy suggests that structural integrity in these regions may play a role in preserving social-cognitive abilities, even within the context of 47,XXX.

The second remarkable finding of this study is the reduced regional volume in the cerebellum in women with 47,XXX karyotype. We are unaware of any other study showing this structural change in individuals with an extra X chromosome. The cerebellum’s regional volume reduction may have implications for movement impairments and tremor. Tremor, in its different forms, has been described in conjunction with 90 chromosome aneuploidies involving sex chromosomes such as Klinefelter syndrome (47,XXY) [59] and Jacobs syndrome (47, XYY) [60]. However, tremor in Triple X syndrome has been occasionally observed but not systematically investigated so far [61]. Interestingly, essential tremor has been associated with altered cerebellar connectivity (for a review, see 62). So far, these are hypotheses, which is why future studies are needed to systematically investigate the proposed link between structural changes in the cerebellum and motor function in women with 47,XXX.

The traditional view that the cerebellum is involved exclusively in movement control and fine motor skills was debunked more than 50 years ago [63]. More recent studies show that the cerebellum is involved in social-cognitive and especially theory-of-mind functions in addition to other cognitive functions [64]. The ability to attribute mental states to other people is impaired, for example, when the cerebellar structure or function is impaired. These impairments are particularly pronounced if cerebellar development is disturbed in early childhood [65]. Symptoms of the autistic spectrum, for example, can be observed as a direct result of cerebellar damage in early childhood [66]. We therefore hypothesize that the regional volume reduction in the cerebellum seen in the present study could at least be a correlate of the partially autism-like social-cognitive and behavioral problems in individuals with 47,XXX karyotype [9, 10]. However, again, more systematic research involving in-depth exploration of the socio-affective and other cognitive functions is needed. In addition, it should be noted that the cerebellum is associated with various functions, so that the functional significance of the structural changes in the context of this study is not clear [67]. Systematic investigations of structural and functional changes in the cerebellum in the context of different cognitive and motor tasks, as already conducted in autism [64], would be desirable to obtain more specific findings in 47,XXX.

In mammals, one of the two X chromosomes in the female sex, if present, is obligatorily inacti-vated. The heterochromatic chromosome is microscopically visible as the so-called “Barr body” in the nucleus. Inactivation is also obligatory in X chromosome aneuploidy syndromes. X-inactivation is facilitated by the non-coding RNA ‘X inactive-specific transcript’, XIST (for review, see 68). However, some genes, such as RNF12 and ABCB7, escape this process, and are regularly expressed from both alleles on the X chromosome to a certain extent. For example, the protein encoded by the murine Rnf12 gene, ubiquitin protein ligase Rnf12, regulates all transcription factor-encoding genes in the LIM homeodomain family [69, 70], such as Lmx1b, which is involved in early cerebellar development [71]. Double-mutant Lmx1b/Lmx1a mice do not develop a cerebellum, a condition associated with ataxia [72]. Other members of the LIM homeodomain gene family are involved in many developmental processes affecting brain regions, among these the hippocampus (for review, see 73). The second gene mentioned above, Abcb7, is causative for X-linked sideroblastic anemia with ataxia in mice [74]. Given the movement impairments (‘clumsiness’) frequently observed in Triple X syndrome (for review, see 3), processes associated with dysregulated X inactivation and X-linked genes might be involved in impaired development of the cerebellum and other brain areas in Triple X syndrome described here, and require more investigation. Serrarens and colleagues [75] recently demonstrated a a supernumerary X chromosome effect in adult-aged women on intracortical myelin across supragranular laminae of the banks of the superior temporal sulcus and inferior temporal gyrus, both involved in language processing [76] and social cognition [77]. These white matter structural alterations may lead to altered signal transduction in these brain areas, and also be associated with the speech delay [78] and social cognitive impairments [7] observed in gonosomal trisomies.

The present study has some limitations to be considered when interpreting our results. Firstly, our sample of 20 women studied is relatively small, but this corresponds to the usual sample sizes in previous studies. Nonetheless, our main results are statistically significant even after the most conservative correction for multiple testing. Secondly, it must be taken into account that, as in all published studies to date, we only conducted a group comparison and not a longitudinal analysis. Strictly speaking, it is therefore not possible to draw conclusions about the significance of these findings in terms of how the structural changes described here develop; these would have to be investigated in similar studies with a dedicated longitudinal design. Thirdly, all interpretations must consider that the 47,XXX clinical picture is very heterogeneous. While a few women complain of major problems in everyday (social) life, many others are significantly less affected, or not affected at all. Therefore, we cannot guarantee that the sample examined here is representative of the entire population. Finally, it is important to note that our voxel-based analyses results were significantly more robust than the surface-based analyses. The results reported on cortical thickness must be regarded as exploratory and preliminary, and are thus not interpreted here. Studies with larger samples, ideally multicenter studies, would be desirable to investigate regional changes in intracortical structure (e.g. cortical thickness, folding, complexity).

Conclusions

In sum, this is the first study using VBM and SBM on 3T structural scans in a sample of adult women with 47,XXX that showed marked regional gray matter volume reductions in limbic areas (in particular amygdala and hippocampus), basal ganglia and other subcortical regions, parts of neocortex and, for the first time, in the cerebellum. These alterations in a distributed network of brain regions involved in social cognition and other cognitive functions might thus help us better understand the neural underpinnings of the behavioral characteristics in 47,XXX.

Data availability

The dataset used and analysed for the current study is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Jacobs P, Baikie AG, Brown WMC, Macgregor TN, Maclean N, Harnden DG. Evidence for the existence of the human super female. Lancet. 1959;274(7100):423–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Berglund A, Viuff MH, Skakkebæk A, Chang S, Stochholm K, Gravholt CH. Changes in the cohort composition of Turner syndrome and severe non-diagnosis of Klinefelter, 47,XXX and 47,XYY syndrome: a nationwide cohort study. Orphanet J Rare Dis. 2019;14(1):16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Otter M, Schrander-Stumpel CT, Curfs LM. Triple X syndrome: a review of the literature. Eur J Hum Genet. 2010;18(3):265–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Tartaglia NR, Howell S, Sutherland A, Wilson R, Wilson L. A review of trisomy X (47,XXX). Orphanet J Rare Dis. 2010;5(1):8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Leggett V, Jacobs P, Nation K, Scerif G, Bishop DVM. Neurocognitive outcomes of individuals with a sex chromosome trisomy: XXX, XYY, or XXY: a systematic review. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2010;52(2):119–29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Stochholm K, Juul S, Gravholt CH. Poor socio-economic status in 47,XXX --an unexpected effect of an extra X chromosome. Eur J Med Genet. 2013;56(6):286–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Urbanus E, Rijn S, Swaab H. A review of neurocognitive functioning of children with sex chromosome trisomies: identifying targets for early intervention. Clin Genet. 2020;97(1):156–67.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. van Rijn S. A review of neurocognitive functioning and risk for psychopathology in sex chromosome trisomy (47,XXY, 47,XXX, 47, XYY). Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2019;32(2):79–84.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Bouw N, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Cordeiro L, van Rijn S. Early social behavior in young children with sex chromosome trisomies (XXX, XXY, XYY): profiles of observed social interactions and social impairments associated with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). J Autism Dev Disord. 2023;53(8):3194–207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Bouw N, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Wilson RL, Van der Velde K, van Rijn S. Early symptoms of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in 1–8 year old children with sex chromosome trisomies (XXX, XXY, XYY), and the predictive value of joint attention. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2023;32(11):2323–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Kuiper K, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, van Rijn S. Early developmental impact of sex chromosome trisomies on attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder symptomology in young children. Am J Med Genet A. 2021;185(12):3664–74.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. van Rijn S, Kuiper K, Bouw N, Urbanus E, Swaab H. Neurocognitive and behavioral development in young children (1–7 years) with sex chromosome trisomy. Endocr Connect. 2023;12(5):e220494.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Wilson AC, King J, Bishop DVM. Autism and social anxiety in children with sex chromosome trisomies: an observational study. Wellcome Open Res. 2019;4:32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Freilinger P, Kliegel D, Hänig S, Oehl-Jaschkowitz B, Henn W, Meyer J. Behavioral and psychological features in girls and women with triple-X syndrome. Am J Med Genet A. 2018;176(11):2284–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Green T, Flash S, Reiss AL. Sex differences in psychiatric disorders: what we can learn from sex chromosome aneuploidies. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44(1):9–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Lee NR, Anand P, Will E, Adeyemi EI, Clasen LS, Blumenthal JD, et al. Everyday executive functions in down syndrome from early childhood to young adulthood: evidence for both unique and shared characteristics compared to youth with sex chromosome trisomy (XXX and XXY). Front Behav Neurosci. 2015;9:264.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. van Rijn S, Swaab H. Executive dysfunction and the relation with behavioral problems in children with 47,XXY and 47,XXX. Genes Brain Behav. 2015;14(2):200–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Bouw N, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Cordeiro L, van Rijn S. The impact of sex chromosome trisomies (XXX, XXY, XYY) on gaze towards faces and affect recognition: a cross-sectional eye tracking study. J Neurodev Disord. 2022;14(1):44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Otter M, Crins PML, Campforts BCM, Stumpel CTRM, van Amelsvoort TAMJ, Vingerhoets C. Social functioning and emotion recognition in adults with triple X syndrome. BJPsych Open. 2021;7(2):e51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. van Rijn S, Stockmann L, Borghgraef M, Bruining H, van Ravenswaaij-Arts C, Govaerts L, et al. The social behavioral phenotype in boys and girls with an extra X chromosome (Klinefelter syndrome and trisomy X): a comparison with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(2):310–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. van Rijn S, Stockmann L, van Buggenhout G, van Ravenswaaij-Arts C, Swaab H. Social cognition and underlying cognitive mechanisms in children with an extra X chromosome: a comparison with autism spectrum disorder. Genes Brain Behav. 2014;13(5):459–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Fish AM, Cachia A, Fischer C, Mankiw C, Reardon PK, Clasen LS, et al. Influences of brain size, sex, and sex chromosome complement on the architecture of human cortical folding. Cereb Cortex. 2017;27(12):5557–67.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Lenroot RK, Blumenthal JD, Wallace GL, Clasen LS, Lee NR, Giedd JN. A case-control study of brain structure and behavioral characteristics in 47,XXX syndrome. Genes Brain Behav. 2014;13(8):841–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Nadig A, Reardon PK, Seidlitz J, McDermott CL, Blumenthal JD, Clasen LS et al. Carriage of supernumerary sex chromosomes decreases the volume and alters the shape of limbic structures. eNeuro. 2018; 5(5):ENEURO.0265-18.2018.

  25. Raznahan A, Lee NR, Greenstein D, Wallace GL, Blumenthal JD, Clasen LS, et al. Globally divergent but locally convergent X- and Y-chromosome influences on cortical development. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26(1):70–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Reardon PK, Clasen L, Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Lerch JP, Chakravarty MM, et al. An allometric analysis of sex and sex chromosome dosage effects on subcortical anatomy in humans. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2016;36(8):2438–48.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Patwardhan AJ, Brown WE, Bender BG, Linden MG, Eliez S, Reiss AL. Reduced size of the amygdala in individuals with 47,XXY and 47,XXX karyotypes. Am J Med Genet. 2002;114(1):93–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Serrarens C, Otter M, Campforts BCM, Stumpel CTRM, Jansma H, Van Amelsvoort TAMJ, et al. Altered subcortical and cortical brain morphology in adult women with 47,XXX: a 7-Tesla magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurodev Disord. 2022;14(1):14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Weiß RH, Weiß B. CFT 20-R - Grundintelligenztest Skala 2 - Revision [Culture Fair Test 20 - Revised]. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Schmidt KH, Metzler P. WST - Wortschatztest [Vocabulary Test]. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Freitag CM, Retz-Junginger P, Retz W, Seitz C, Palmason H, Meyer J, et al. Evaluation der Deutschen version des Autismus-Spektrum-Quotienten (AQ) - die Kurzversion AQ-k [Evaluation of the German version of the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) - the short version AQ-k]. Z Für Klin Psychol Psychother. 2007;36(4):280–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The Autism-Spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from asperger Syndrome/High-Functioning autism, Malesand females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31(1):5–17.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1983;44(1):113–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Laux L, Glanzmann P, Schaffner P, Spielberger CD. STAI - Das State-Trait-Angstinventar [State-Trait anxiety inventory]. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Hautzinger M, Keller F, Kühner C. BDI-II - Beck - depressions - Inventar– [Beck depression inventory II]. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Gaser C, Dahnke R, Thompson PM, Kurth F, Luders E, Initiative ADN. CAT– A Computational Anatomy Toolbox for the Analysis of Structural MRI Data [Internet]. bioRxiv; 2023 [cited 2024 May 16]. Available from: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1101/2022.06.11.495736v2

  37. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Voxel-based morphometry–the methods. NeuroImage. 2000;11(6 Pt 1):805–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Rolls ET, Huang C-C, Lin C-P, Feng J, Joliot M. Automated anatomical labelling atlas 3. NeuroImage. 2020;206:116189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Rorden C, Brett M. Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. Behav Neurol. 2000;12(4):191–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage. 2006;31(3):968–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. de Medeiros Marcos GVT, Feitosa DDM, Paiva KM, Oliveira RF, Rocha GS, da M, de Guerra LM, et al. Volumetric alterations in the basal ganglia in autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2024;84(3):163–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Nickl-Jockschat T, Habel U, Michel TM, Manning J, Laird AR, Fox PT, et al. Brain structure anomalies in autism spectrum disorder–a meta-analysis of VBM studies using anatomic likelihood Estimation. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33(6):1470–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Morawetz C, Basten U. Neural underpinnings of individual differences in emotion regulation: A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2024;162:105727.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Tso IF, Rutherford S, Fang Y, Angstadt M, Taylor SF. The social brain is highly sensitive to the Mere presence of social information: an automated meta-analysis and an independent study. PLoS ONE. 2018;13(5):e0196503.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Zinchenko O, Yaple ZA, Arsalidou M. Brain responses to dynamic facial expressions: a normative meta-analysis. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018;12:227.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. DeMayo MM, Young LJ, Hickie IB, Song YJC, Guastella AJ. Circuits for social learning: A unified model and application to autism spectrum disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;107:388–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Del Casale A, Ferracuti S, Alcibiade A, Simone S, Modesti MN, Pompili M. Neuroanatomical correlates of autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis of structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2022;325:111516.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Otter M, Campforts BCM, Stumpel CTRM, van Amelsvoort TAMJ, Drukker M. Triple X syndrome: psychiatric disorders and impaired social functioning as a risk factor. Eur Psychiatry J Assoc Eur Psychiatr. 2022;66(1):e7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Schulze L, Schmahl C, Niedtfeld I. Neural correlates of disturbed emotion processing in borderline personality disorder: a multimodal meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;79(2):97–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Ho NF, Chong PLH, Lee DR, Chew QH, Chen G, Sim K. The amygdala in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a synthesis of structural MRI, diffusion tensor imaging, and resting-state functional connectivity findings. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2019;27(3):150–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Liang J, Yu Q, Liu Y, Qiu Y, Tang R, Yan L, et al. Gray matter abnormalities in patients with major depressive disorder and social anxiety disorder: a voxel-based meta-analysis. Brain Imaging Behav. 2023;17(6):749–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Bird G, Viding E. The self to other model of empathy: providing a new framework for Understanding empathy impairments in psychopathy, autism, and alexithymia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;47:520–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Marsh AA. The neuroscience of empathy. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2018;19:110–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Decety J. The neural pathways, development and functions of empathy. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2015;3:1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Fatima M, Babu N. Cognitive and affective empathy in autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis. Rev J Autism Dev Disord. 2023; 1–20.

  56. Harmsen IE. Empathy in autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49:3939–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Phelps EA. Emotion and cognition: insights from studies of the human amygdala. Annu Rev Psychol. 2006;57(1):27–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Rubin RD, Watson PD, Duff MC, Cohen NJ. The role of the hippocampus in flexible cognition and social behavior. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:742.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Koegl-Wallner M, Katschnig-Winter P, Pendl T, Melisch B, Trummer M, Holl E, et al. Tremor associated with Klinefelter syndrome–a case series and review of the literature. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2014;20(3):323–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Bardsley MZ, Kowal K, Levy C, Gosek A, Ayari N, Tartaglia N, et al. 47,XYY syndrome: clinical phenotype and timing of ascertainment. J Pediatr. 2013;163(4):1085–94.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Garcia X, Mohammad ME, Fernandez H, Itin I, Triple X. syndrome: Are tremors part of its phenotype? [Internet]. 2016 [cited 2024 May 31]. Available from: https://www.mdsabstracts.org/abstract/triple-x-syndrome-are-tremors-part-of-its-phenotype/

  62. van den Berg KRE, Helmich RC. The role of the cerebellum in tremor - evidence from neuroimaging. Tremor Hyperkinetic Mov. 2021;11:49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Dow RS. Some novel concepts of cerebellar physiology. Mt Sinai J Med N Y. 1974;41(1):103–19.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  64. Van Overwalle F, Baetens K, Mariën P, Vandekerckhove M. Social cognition and the cerebellum: A meta-analysis of over 350 fMRI studies. NeuroImage. 2014;86:554–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Olson IR, Hoffman LJ, Jobson KR, Popal HS, Wang Y. Little brain, little Minds: the big role of the cerebellum in social development. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2023;60:101238.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. D’Mello AM, Stoodley CJ. Cerebro-cerebellar circuits in autism spectrum disorder. Front Neurosci. 2015; 9.

  67. Strick PL, Dum RP, Fiez JA. Cerebellum and nonmotor function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32(1):413–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Disteche CM, Berletch JB. X-chromosome inactivation and escape. J Genet. 2015;94(4):591–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Borensztein M, Syx L, Ancelin K, Diabangouaya P, Picard C, Liu T, et al. Xist-dependent imprinted X inactivation and the early developmental consequences of its failure. Nat Struct Mol Biol. 2017;24(3):226–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Ostendorff HP, Bossenz M, Mincheva A, Copeland NG, Gilbert DJ, Jenkins NA, et al. Functional characterization of the gene encoding RLIM, the corepressor of LIM homeodomain factors. Genomics. 2000;69(1):120–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Guo C, Qiu H-Y, Huang Y, Chen H, Yang R-Q, Chen S-D, et al. Lmx1b is essential for Fgf8 and Wnt1 expression in the isthmic organizer during tectum and cerebellum development in mice. Dev Camb Engl. 2007;134(2):317–25.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  72. Chizhikov VV, Iskusnykh IY, Fattakhov N, Fritzsch B. Lmx1a and Lmx1b are redundantly required for the development of multiple components of the mammalian auditory system. Neuroscience. 2021;452:247–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Hobert O, Westphal H. Functions of LIM-homeobox genes. Trends Genet. 2000;16(2):75–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Pondarre C, Campagna DR, Antiochos B, Sikorski L, Mulhern H, Fleming MD. Abcb7, the gene responsible for X-linked sideroblastic anemia with ataxia, is essential for hematopoiesis. Blood. 2007;109(8):3567–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Serrarens C, Ruiz-Fernandez J, Otter M, Campforts BCM, Stumpel CTRM, Linden DEJ, et al. Intracortical Myelin across laminae in adult individuals with 47,XXX: a 7 Tesla MRI study. Cereb Cortex. 2024;34(8):bhae343.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Abrams DA, Kochalka J, Bhide S, Ryali S, Menon V. Intrinsic functional architecture of the human speech processing network. Cortex. 2020;129:41–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Van Overwalle F. Social cognition and the brain: A meta-analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30(3):829–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Schwemmle C, Jungheim M, Ptok M. [Gonosomal trisomy syndrome. Five case reports and review of literature]. Laryngorhinootologie. 2013;92(11):725–31.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank all women for participating in this study. We further thank Birgit Bungert and Robert Gollmick for carrying out the scanning sessions, and Laura Litke and Hendrik Jürgenlimke for their support during the scanning sessions and the psychometric assessments.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.

The publication was in part supported by the Open Access Fund of the University of Trier and by the German Research Foundation (DFG).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

G.D., M.C. and J.M. designed the study. M.C., A.B. and P.F. recruited the participants and conducted data collection. G.D. and M.C. analyzed the data. G.D. wrote the main manuscript text and prepared figures and tables. M.C., A.B., J.M. and W.A.W. substantively revised the manuscript. All authors reviewed and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gregor Domes.

Ethics declarations

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethical approval and consent to participate

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the state medical association Rhineland-Palatinate (#2022–16572). Participants gave written-informed consent before participation.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Domes, G., Croyé, MA., Freilinger, P. et al. Brain structure in triple X syndrome: regional gray matter volume and cortical thickness in adult women with 47,XXX karyotype. J Neurodevelop Disord 17, 18 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s11689-025-09608-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s11689-025-09608-6

Keywords